
The present article is a theoretical contribution suggesting a simple model of the dynamics involved in the development of neurodevelopmental disorders and the phenomenon of autism offering an explanation of why different individuals have different onset of manifest disease. The model relates to present genetic and epigenetic evidence and previous theoretical models. The dynamic model applies an individualized transdiagnostic and dimensional approach integrating several levels of influence involved in the dynamic interaction between an individual and their environment across time. The dynamic model illustrates the interaction between a basic neurobiological susceptibility, compensating mechanisms, and stress-related releasing mechanisms involved in the development of manifest clinical illness. The model has a particular focus on the dynamics of neurocognitive processes and their relationship to more basic information, psychological and social processes. A basic assumption guiding the model is that even quite normal events related to typical development may increase the risk of enduring stress in cognitively vulnerable individuals, further increasing their risk of developing manifest clinical illness. The model suggests that genetic variation, endogenous epigenetic processes of development, and epigenetic changes influenced by physical/chemical and social environmental risk and resilience factors all may contribute to phenotypical expression. A genetic susceptibility may translate into a biological susceptibility reflected in cognitive impairments. A cognitively vulnerable individual may be at increased risk of misinterpreting sensory inputs, potentially resulting in psychopathological expressions, e.g., autistic symptoms, observed across several neurodevelopmental disorders. The genetically influenced experience of an individual relates to cognitive phenomena occurring at the interfaces between the brain, mind, and society. The severity of clinical illness may differ from the severity of a disorder of reasoning. The dynamic model may help guide future development of personalized medicine in psychiatry and identify relevant points of intervention. A discussion of the implications of the model relating to epigenetic evidence and to previous theoretical models is included at the end of the paper.
Citation: Bodil Aggernæs. Suggestion of a dynamic model of the development of neurodevelopmental disorders and the phenomenon of autism[J]. AIMS Molecular Science, 2020, 7(2): 122-182. doi: 10.3934/molsci.2020008
[1] | Matthew S. Sherwood, Jason G. Parker, Emily E. Diller, Subhashini Ganapathy, Kevin Bennett, Jeremy T. Nelson . Volitional down-regulation of the primary auditory cortex via directed attention mediated by real-time fMRI neurofeedback. AIMS Neuroscience, 2018, 5(3): 179-199. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.3.179 |
[2] | Ioannis K Gallos, Kostakis Gkiatis, George K Matsopoulos, Constantinos Siettos . ISOMAP and machine learning algorithms for the construction of embedded functional connectivity networks of anatomically separated brain regions from resting state fMRI data of patients with Schizophrenia. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(2): 295-321. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021016 |
[3] | Zoha Deldar, Carlos Gevers-Montoro, Ali Khatibi, Ladan Ghazi-Saidi . The interaction between language and working memory: a systematic review of fMRI studies in the past two decades. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(1): 1-32. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021001 |
[4] | Aini Ismafairus Abd Hamid, Nurfaten Hamzah, Siti Mariam Roslan, Nur Alia Amalin Suhardi, Muhammad Riddha Abdul Rahman, Faiz Mustafar, Hazim Omar, Asma Hayati Ahmad, Elza Azri Othman, Ahmad Nazlim Yusoff . Distinct neural mechanisms of alpha binaural beats and white noise for cognitive enhancement in young adults. AIMS Neuroscience, 2025, 12(2): 147-179. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2025010 |
[5] | Evangelos Almpanis, Constantinos Siettos . Construction of functional brain connectivity networks from fMRI data with driving and modulatory inputs: an extended conditional Granger causality approach. AIMS Neuroscience, 2020, 7(2): 66-88. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2020005 |
[6] | Paul G. Nestor, Toshiyuki Ohtani, James J. Levitt, Dominick T. Newell, Martha E. Shenton, Margaret Niznikiewicz, Robert W. McCarley . Prefrontal Lobe Gray Matter, Cognitive Control and Episodic Memory in Healthy Cognition. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(3): 338-355. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.3.338 |
[7] | Charles R Larson, Donald A Robin . Sensory Processing: Advances in Understanding Structure and Function of Pitch-Shifted Auditory Feedback in Voice Control. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(1): 22-39. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.1.22 |
[8] | Byron Bernal, Alfredo Ardila, Monica Rosselli . The Network of Brodmanns Area 22 in Lexico-semantic Processing: A Pooling-data Connectivity Study. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(3): 306-316. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.3.306 |
[9] | Kevin Pierre, Vanessa Molina, Shil Shukla, Anthony Avila, Nicholas Fong, Jessica Nguyen, Brandon Lucke-Wold . Chronic traumatic encephalopathy: Diagnostic updates and advances. AIMS Neuroscience, 2022, 9(4): 519-535. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2022030 |
[10] | Anna Lardone, Marianna Liparoti, Pierpaolo Sorrentino, Roberta Minino, Arianna Polverino, Emahnuel Troisi Lopez, Simona Bonavita, Fabio Lucidi, Giuseppe Sorrentino, Laura Mandolesi . Topological changes of brain network during mindfulness meditation: an exploratory source level magnetoencephalographic study. AIMS Neuroscience, 2022, 9(2): 250-263. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2022013 |
The present article is a theoretical contribution suggesting a simple model of the dynamics involved in the development of neurodevelopmental disorders and the phenomenon of autism offering an explanation of why different individuals have different onset of manifest disease. The model relates to present genetic and epigenetic evidence and previous theoretical models. The dynamic model applies an individualized transdiagnostic and dimensional approach integrating several levels of influence involved in the dynamic interaction between an individual and their environment across time. The dynamic model illustrates the interaction between a basic neurobiological susceptibility, compensating mechanisms, and stress-related releasing mechanisms involved in the development of manifest clinical illness. The model has a particular focus on the dynamics of neurocognitive processes and their relationship to more basic information, psychological and social processes. A basic assumption guiding the model is that even quite normal events related to typical development may increase the risk of enduring stress in cognitively vulnerable individuals, further increasing their risk of developing manifest clinical illness. The model suggests that genetic variation, endogenous epigenetic processes of development, and epigenetic changes influenced by physical/chemical and social environmental risk and resilience factors all may contribute to phenotypical expression. A genetic susceptibility may translate into a biological susceptibility reflected in cognitive impairments. A cognitively vulnerable individual may be at increased risk of misinterpreting sensory inputs, potentially resulting in psychopathological expressions, e.g., autistic symptoms, observed across several neurodevelopmental disorders. The genetically influenced experience of an individual relates to cognitive phenomena occurring at the interfaces between the brain, mind, and society. The severity of clinical illness may differ from the severity of a disorder of reasoning. The dynamic model may help guide future development of personalized medicine in psychiatry and identify relevant points of intervention. A discussion of the implications of the model relating to epigenetic evidence and to previous theoretical models is included at the end of the paper.
Functional brain imaging has become an important resource for understanding brain function and neuropsychiatric conditions. Each neuroimaging tool has its own strengths and limitations, and it is generally agreed that different modalities may contribute complimentary information, such as electrophysiology vs. blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signals and structural connectivity vs. functional connectivity (FC) [1]. In contrast to the low spatial resolution of electroencephalography, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) may provide neuro-informatics at higher anatomical precision from tens to hundreds of thousands of voxels. An innate difficulty associated with fMRI lies in how to utilize the high dimensional data to draw conclusions.
It is well known that the temporal dynamics between neighboring fMRI voxels are not totally independent of each other, justifying an application of a reduction strategy to simplify the data structure. In activation designs (i.e., the subjects are challenged with psychological tasks), both parametric (random field theory) and non-parametric (simulation) approaches have been developed to quantify “resolution elements” that may help to eschew stringent Bonferroni correction and hence to provide appropriate statistical inferences [2],[3]. However, in resting state fMRI (rs-fMRI) designs, researchers still heavily rely on predefined regions of interest (ROIs) that are informed by anatomical atlases, not the BOLD data itself, to achieve data reduction (i.e., structural parcellation) [4]–[9]. The resolution of rs-fMRI data can therefore be reduced to tens or hundreds of units. Although widely applied, its validity has been questioned because the correspondence between functionally specialized brain regions and brain areas delineated by anatomical landmarks and cytoarchitectonic structures may be limited [4],[5],[10]–[13]. In other words, the ROIs derived from structural atlases are not necessarily functionally homogeneous, which may result in biased and even faulty network property estimation. Empirical evidence arguing both for and against the aforementioned approach has been reported [4],[5],[7]–[9],[12],[13]. A recent study suggested that despite the flawed biological plausibility, whether an atlas-informed partition of the cortex is a valid data-reduction strategy or not is actually a context-dependent issue [6]. Either taking its validity for granted or rejecting its usefulness blatantly is an arbitrary assertion.
Given the ambiguity and debate described above, it is desirable to parcellate the cerebral cortex based on the similarity of regional and neighboring BOLD dynamics itself to achieve data reduction (i.e., functional parcellation [FP]), instead of resorting to the structural parcellation via pre-defined ROIs. However, this challenging issue in rs-fMRI research is more complicated than it appears. The cortex is organized in distributed hierarchical and parallel manners with intense forward, backward and lateral projections [14]–[16], which may endorse diverse functional specialization of brain regions and sub-regions [17]–[27]. The above observation indicates that FP of the cortex can actually be performed at multiple resolutions that are dependent on the perspectives/scales that researchers wish to explore [28],[29]. However, contradictory evidence suggests that the cortical modules in rs-fMRI research seem to be stable and independent of different methodologies [4],[30]. Although several elegant algorithms have been developed to perform automatic whole-brain FP, a consensus has not been reached yet [31]–[35]. Previous research sampled the cortex uniformly to reduce the data dimensions [28]. Cohen et al. utilized information of the transition of FC maps to delineate the boundaries between functional areas, while Wig et al. applied snowball sampling to identify the neighbors and neighbors of neighbors and so forth to achieve FP [31],[34]. Although the above exemplified methods are persuasive in formulation and comparable in results, they provide fixed partitions and do not address the nature of local homogeneity that is the fundamental observation and rationale to pursue a data-reduction procedure (note: “local homogeneity” is used to avoid confusion with the term “regional homogeneity” in re-fMRI research, ReHo).
This study proposes a fast, iterative algorithm to perform automatic FP that incorporates modular analysis and similarity measurements (named MOSI), the former splitting a module into sub-modules and the latter uniting similar sub-modules into a bigger module. Modules or communities are groups of nodes within a network that are more densely connected to one another than to other nodes [36],[37]. In this research, the neural nodes are the voxels, and the connectivity strengths (edges) between nodes are defined by FC, i.e., correlation strength. Since the (sub-) modules were derived by the connectivity strengths, the voxels with similar BOLD dynamics (thus higher connectivity strengths) tended to aggregate as a community. The modularity of a partition is a metric between −1 and 1 that indicates the quality of the network partition, i.e., the density of edges inside communities compared to that of edges between communities [37],[38]. After partition, the mean BOLD signal of each (sub-) module is correlated with those of the remaining (sub-) modules to constitute a correlation (FC) map. The adjacent (sub-) modules with similar FC maps (and hence smaller “relative distance”) are grouped together as a new module (note: adjacency is defined as 26 possible connections by any points, lines or faces of a 3-dimensional cube). Iteration of the division and unification processes will be carried out until the derived modular structure is stable. The number of modules and, hence, resolution of a partition can be titrated by gamma values (see Material and Methods for details). Since information processing in the cerebral cortex relies on interactions among the distributed areas, the results of FP are subject to further exploration to disclose the constituent communities, which are compared with known brain networks [17]–[19],[35].
For detailed information refer to Lee et al.'s previous paper [39], which is briefly summarized below. Thirty-three right-handed healthy subjects were selected from the dataset of the Rockland sample (Enhanced Nathan Kline Institute; NKI-RS), with a mean age of 26.6 years. The data source used by this study met the criteria of “NKI-RS Neuroimaging Data Release”. The institutional ethics committee approved this project. T1-weighted structural MRI (sMRI) and rs-fMRI were included in the analysis. All MRI images covered the whole brain and were acquired by a 3.0 Tesla Siemens Magnetom Trio Tim system, with 260 whole-brain echoplanar images (EPIs) and TR 2.5 seconds (first 5 images were discarded for equilibrium).
FreeSurfer was applied to the T1-weighted sMRI to segment gray matter and white matter and to parcellate the cortical mantle into 70 (Desikan-Killiany Atlas) ROIs [40],[41]. The Analysis of Functional NeuroImages software package (AFNI) was used to process the rs-fMRI data [3], and the analytic streamline developed by Jo et al. was adopted to prepare the rs-fMRI images [4],[5],[42]. The pre-processing steps of rs-fMRI comprised despike, slice-time correction, realignment (motion corrected), registration to T1 anatomy, spatial smoothing (6 mm), and bandpass filtering to 0.01–0.1 Hz. Several regressors were created and modeled as nuisance variables, comprising twelve movement parameters, tissue-based regressors of white matter (global and local) and ventricles, and third-order polynomials to fit baseline drift [42]. The tissue-based regressors were constructed with the help of the segmentation results obtained from FreeSurfer.
MOSI is an iterative process, and each iteration mainly comprises two steps to divide and to merge modules. It is notable that different FC maps were constructed by Pearson's correlation for each step (i.e., regional and global, detailed below). In the division stage, the Louvain community detection algorithm was applied to each module derived from the preceding iteration. The first iteration starts from the ROIs of the cortex delineated from the Desikan-Killiany Atlas. In this stage of MOSI, the “regional” correlation map of each module was generated as an input to the Louvain algorithm for community detection (see next section). Suppose a particular module has N voxels, then the correlation map is a matrix with dimension N × N, with each element being the correlation coefficient (CC) of a voxel-pair within that module.
It is noteworthy that although several community detection algorithms can be applied directly to a huge network—the whole brain in this case—the resultant network patterns are inevitably composed of distant brain regions, which is in accord with the fact that the brain is an intensely interactive system [4],[5],[45]. However, what is more important for automatic FP is to emphasize local (neighboring) “homogeneity” in terms of the BOLD dynamics. Another application of community detection in previous rs-fMRI research is to decompose a brain region into several sub-regions [39],[46],[47]. At this stage, MOSI is similar to the latter. If the voxels of a resultant sub-module form disconnected sub-groups, say 2 sub-groups, this disconnected sub-module is further separated into 2 smaller communities because only neighboring voxels are qualified to be grouped as a module in MOSI. Different resolutions were explored by setting gamma values from 0.65 to 0.95 at an interval of 0.05.
The second stage is to unite similar modules into a larger module. In this unification stage of MOSI, the mean time series of each module was calculated, which was correlated with the remaining modules to generate another correlation map (a vector). This FC vector carried the sampled information of the entire cerebral cortex that was used to define similarity, thus a “global” FC map [6]. The similarity among the FC maps was computed for each pair of neighboring modules, which is the square root of the weighted square sum of the difference between two FC maps (weight reflects the voxel number). Similarity was indicated by the relative distance of two FC maps, which was calculated for each pair of neighboring modules. For a similarity index less than 0.05, neighboring modules were unified to become a new module. In other words, if the mean BOLD signals of two neighboring modules showed a high correlation but their (whole-brain) FC vector looked different, they were regarded as separated and not unified, which was expected to possess higher differentiability. It is notable that the entire cortex information is sampled to define relative distance. A large relative distance means that two brain FC maps show substantial differences.
The program ends when the modular structure is stable. Operationally, stop criteria can be evaluated from two perspectives: the output modular structures for two consecutive cycles are either exactly the same or their normalized mutual information is higher than 0.95, in which a 1 percent difference in modular number was allowed. The mean CCs of each resolution of the MOSI results were summarized (Table 1), and the mean CCs were compared across different resolutions and with those in the original ROIs (Table 2). Each hemisphere was treated separately. SUMA (AFNI associated software) was used to illustrate the MOSI partition results.
There are several technical details worthy of mentioning. First, in the beginning of each iteration step, small/fragmented modules with a voxel number less than 10 were identified and united with neighboring modules that showed the strongest correlation strength of no less than 0.50. Tiny modules inherently had fewer neighbors and were believed to be part of a larger neighboring community because of the innate covariance structure of fMRI data and the applied smoothing procedure [2],[3]. Considering the possible number of combinations between (sub-) modules, the computation time for each iteration increases drastically with modular number. This procedure may reduce the number of modules that are to be treated and hence save computation time. Second, for each iteration of MOSI, a permutation is introduced to the order of the modules before computation. Take modules A, B and C as an example. After partitioning, suppose that A is decomposed into A1 and A2, B into B1 and B2, and C into C1 and C2. A1 shows the highest similarity to B2, while B2 shows the highest similarity to C1. If changing the order does not occur, A1 will always be united with B2, and B2 will have no chance to unite with C1—this could not be the optimal solution. This permutation may facilitate the algorithm to converge. Last, since this algorithm works quite quickly for lower values of parameter gamma (e.g., ≤0.8), if the users wish to perform FP at a higher resolution, it is recommended to run MOSI at a lower gamma value to provide a good initial guess as a starting point. In other words, a finer FP is derived from a coarser FP instead of breaking the original ROIs into many small modules and repeating the splitting-unification cycles for the high gamma condition. In this study, the results of gamma 0.70 are based on the results of gamma 0.65, and the results of gamma 0.75 are based on the results of gamma 0.70 and so forth. This simple strategy may substantially decrease computation time.
The Louvain method was employed to partition each module into composite sub-modules through optimization of Newman's modularity metric Q [36]–[38], in which , Aij is the connectivity strength between nodes i and j, ci is the community that node i belongs to, δ is the delta function (if ci = cj, delta = 1, otherwise 0), and γ (gamma) is the resolution factor. The Louvain algorithm may accommodate different resolutions by adjusting gamma values and may decompose parametric/weighted networks (in contrast to binary networks in which the connections are either 0 or 1) [43],[44]. A higher gamma value means that the resultant partitions are smaller in size (fewer nodes), and vice versa. The output partitions of MOSI can be compared by normalized mutual information; if two partitions are exactly the same, their mutual information will be one. The above computations of community detection and community comparison were performed using the brain connectivity toolbox (http://www.brain-connectivity-toolbox.net) [37]. The mathematical software Matlab was the platform used to execute the brain connectivity toolbox and was used to manage the MOSI pipeline (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA).
The BOLD time series of the voxels in each MOSI-derived module are expected to be more similar when compared with those in other modules. The validity was examined by an intra-individual analysis in which the mean CC between the voxels within each module was compared with the (averaged) CC between the mean BOLD signals of that module and other modules, with the former expected to be greater than the latter. The null hypothesis was that the CC within each module was the same as the CC between different modules (i.e., the mean difference is zero), and a t-test was performed for each subject.
To assess the influence of resolution, grand mean CCs across seven gamma values for each participant were calculated by taking the mean of CCs from the BOLD dynamics of the voxels of the constituent modules. Similar calculations were applied to the initial partition of the 70 ROIs. Given that Louvain partition is based on the correlation strengths and the neighboring voxels of fMRI tend to be highly correlated, it is inferred that the MOSI-derived modules would show higher intra-modular similarity with finer resolution (i.e., with increasing gamma value, the derived modules shall be smaller in size and with more homogeneous content if MOSI works properly).
The results of MOSI provide a novel starting point to identify networks of functionally coupled regions across the cerebral cortex. The mean representative BOLD dynamics were retrieved to achieve data reduction, and the module was shrunk to a neural node. A new set of FC maps (second level) were constructed accordingly, and the community detection algorithm was applied again to the simplified neuro-informatics to disclose underlying modular structures – large-scale networks. The number of output networks were arbitrarily assigned to 9 by adjusting the parameters of Louvain algorithm. The detailed modular structures derived across 7 gamma values were compared with 5 commonly reported networks in previous rs-fMRI research [28],[48], i.e., visuo-associative, cognitive/attentional fronto-parietal, default-mode, sensorimotor and limbic/paralimbic networks (note: the attentional fronto-parietal network is not a robust finding, which is not always mentioned in previous reports [6],[30],[45]). The results were visualized by AFNI and SUMA.
The validity of MOSI was examined by t-tests for each subject (n = 33), hemisphere (n = 2) and gamma value (n = 7), and the P values were generally less than 10−3, and t-stats were greater than 4.08 (414 out of the 33*2*7 [=462] conditions had P values < 10−3). The results verify that MOSI reliably performs FP based on local homogeneity in terms of BOLD dynamics. It was discovered that the grand mean CCs increased with gamma values. The grand mean CC of the original partition informed by the Desikan-Killiany atlas was 0.33 (s.d. 0.034), which increased to 0.60 (s.d. 0.015) when gamma was 0.95. The resolution at gamma 0.65 was approximately 20 modules for each hemisphere, which increased to 170 modules when gamma reached 0.95; please see Table 1 and Figure 1 for details.
Paired t-tests of the grand mean CCs across all of the gamma values showed that the grand mean CC for a higher gamma value was always significantly greater than that for a lower gamma value, with most of the P-values of the pair-wise comparisons less than 10−4 (Bonferroni correction of P-value 0.05 is 0.05/28 pairs = 0.0018); please refer to Table 2 for details. For the above statistical comparisons, Fisher's transformation of CCs was also applied, and the conclusions remained the same (data thus not shown). It was found that MOSI worked efficiently, and the average calculation time was less than 6 minutes for gamma values no greater than 0.85 (Table 1; Memory 15.5 GiB, Processor Intel® Core™ i7-4700HQ CPU@2.40GHz, Disk capacity 1.0 TB). However, when gamma values were 0.90 and 0.95, the respective calculation time was approximately 8.5 and 26 minutes, indicating that the computation time of MOSI increased drastically when the cortex of a hemisphere was partitioned into greater than 90 communities (ROIs). A partition derived from different gamma values is illustrated using SUMA in Figure 2.
The MOSI results provide a novel start for data reduction of fMRI signals. Abridged (second level) FC maps were constructed accordingly (using the mean to represent all the voxels in a module and calculating the correlation matrix between the constituent modules). Another set of modular analyses were carried out consequently, revealing an interesting pattern. The authors attempted to identify the 5 commonly reported networks across the seven gamma values for each subject (i.e., visuo-associative, cognitive/attentional fronto-parietal, default-mode, sensorimotor and limbic/paralimbic networks); see an illustration by AFNI in Figure 3. There is inter-individual variation in terms of the spatial distribution of the constituent modules; however, the delineated modular structures are consistent and prominent, which is summarized in Table 3. With respect to conventional large-scale brain networks, the results suggest that a gamma value between 0.8 and 0.85 is reasonably good for individual analysis.
gamma | mean | s.d. | number (L/R) | time (sec) |
original | 0.33 | 0.034 | 25/25 | n/a |
0.65 | 0.40 | 0.036 | 20.2/19.5 | 124.0 |
0.70 | 0.41 | 0.040 | 23.2/23.4 | 199.2 |
0.75 | 0.41 | 0.036 | 26.9/26.9 | 317.8 |
0.80 | 0.43 | 0.026 | 38.6/37.6 | 337.7 |
0.85 | 0.47 | 0.022 | 55.3/54.2 | 359.5 |
0.90 | 0.53 | 0.018 | 91.7/91.4 | 511.7 |
0.95 | 0.60 | 0.015 | 169.3/169.3 | 1542.5 |
gamma | original | 0.65 | 0.70 | 0.75 | 0.80 | 0.85 | 0.90 | 0.95 |
original | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 0.13 | 0.19 | 0.26 | |
0.65 | 12.8 | 0.01* | 0.01* | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.14 | 0.21 | |
0.70 | 15.6 | 2.1* | 0.00* | 0.02 | 0.06 | 0.12 | 0.19 | |
0.75 | 17.4 | 2.8* | 0.9* | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.11 | 0.17 | |
0.80 | 25.0 | 6.8 | 5.3 | 5.9 | 0.04 | 0.10 | 0.13 | |
0.85 | 31.4 | 13.7 | 12.3 | 14.5 | 14.8 | 0.06 | 0.07 | |
0.90 | 34.9 | 23.8 | 20.0 | 21.3 | 25.3 | 18.9 | ||
0.95 | 47.8 | 36.2 | 31.0 | 33.7 | 43.6 | 41.9 | 31.0 |
Note: the sign of the number is ignored because the grand mean CC for higher gamma values is always higher than that of the lower gamma values. For all the pair-wise comparisons, the
Network Scoring: 1, present; 0.5, present but defective (e.g., showing only one side of network A, and the other side of network A being confluent with network B, etc.); 0, absent. This arbitrary scoring system obeys two simple rules. First, the priority follows the order SM > FP > DM > VA > LP. For example, if SM is confluent with DM, then DM is 0, and SM is 1. If DM is divided into two parts and one part is united with SM, the score of DM would be 0.5. Integrity is the second consideration. For example, if FP is divided into several small parts, then its priority over DM, VA and LP is lost. However, if with increasing gamma, DM is divided into anterior and posterior components and neither is merged with other networks, then the score of DM is still 1.
Abbreviation: visuo-associative, VA; default-mode, DM; sensorimotor, SM; fronto-parietal executive, FP; limbic/paralimbic, LP.
gamma | VA | DM | SM | FP | LP |
0.65 | 83 | 79 | 87 | 80 | 62 |
0.70 | 88 | 85 | 90 | 78 | 82 |
0.75 | 89 | 89 | 99 | 77 | 88 |
0.80 | 85 | 95 | 87 | 83 | 86 |
0.85 | 87 | 95 | 98 | 83 | 81 |
0.90 | 91 | 97 | 94 | 96 | 69 |
0.95 | 86 | 96 | 94 | 95 | 64 |
It is believed that brain tissues that perform similar functions tend to aggregate together. Homogeneous cortical regions have been described using various tools, such as cytoarchitecture, white matter bundles, and anatomical landmarks. Given that the voxel number is large in fMRI (whole brain scanning may involve tens to hundreds of thousands of voxels) and the BOLD dynamics of voxels are not totally independent, it is justifiable to partition the cortex into communities with the content voxels sharing similar neural features. This process of dimensionality reduction will result in simplified neuro-informatics that are more feasible for further inferences about brain network properties. Given their theoretical and clinical importance, the research field of automatic FP of the cortex has been versatile, and various innovations have been used to segment the cortex automatically based on the functional characteristics of scanned voxels [28],[49]–[55]. Unlike previously proposed algorithms, MOSI utilizes the concordance in BOLD dynamics of the regional and neighboring voxels to parcellate the functional neuroimaging data. MOSI fulfills automatic segmentation of the cortex by combining modularity detection (dividing step) and similarity measurements (unifying step) that are repeated iteratively until convergence is reached. Using a Louvain community detection algorithm, the gamma value of the modularity formula may affect the resolution of the resultant parcellation [43],[44]. This feature is welcome because it is acknowledged that the central nervous system is organized at multiple spatial scales, ranging from neuron, tissue, gyrus, and area to lobe (or from column and map to system) [31],[56],[57]. In opposition to previous parcellation schemes that tended to yield fixed partition [32]–[35], MOSI may provide graded multi-scale solutions to cortical FP contingent upon research interest. The mean partition number for gamma values of 0.65–0.95 ranges from 40–340 (see Table 1). Intra-subject analyses consistently verified that the BOLD signals in each parcellated module by MOSI are more homogeneous than those in other modules. As expected, the homogeneity in terms of BOLD similarity within each module increases with higher gamma values (see Table 2).
A prerequisite for extracting reliable representative brain signals is to address the regional correlated BOLD signal structure. It is more comfortable to reduce the voxels with homogeneous content into a neural node and to use the mean time series to represent the brain signals embedded in those voxels. The data reduction achieved by MOSI, i.e., shrinking similar voxels to a node, provides a new starting point for subsequent network analysis. It is well known that neural activities in different brain regions are not independent from each other, which is also true for the BOLD dynamics of fMRI data. Investigation into distant neural synchronization has elucidated several large-scale networks in the brain [7],[39],[58], i.e., the brain has stable patterns of spontaneous activity that are organized as modular structures even in the resting state. After MOSI, a community detection algorithm was used again to unveil the hidden organization of the network structure, similar to many previous reports [6],[30],[45],[59]. It is noteworthy that under such a simplified scenario, community detection is used to categorize distantly linked neural nodes, which is in contrast to the analytic stream in MOSI where community detection is used to aggregate homogeneous and neighboring voxels. Regarding the 5 commonly reported large-scale networks, it was found that these network components can be identified at the individual level (except the attentional fronto-parietal network that may be confluent with the executive fronto-parietal network; see Tables 3 and Figure 3). Based on these results, we suggest that gamma values of 0.80–0.85 are suitable resolution for FP of rs-fMRI data, which corresponds to segmenting the cortex into 76–110 patches. Lower gamma values may help to study the interacting features between the networks listed above, while higher gamma values may help to unveil the neural features of the constituent sub-networks. Referring to the anatomical delineation of the limbic-paralimbic system [60], it is interesting to note that the derived limbic/paralimbic component occupied a much smaller region in previous reports and in this report, which was restricted to the orbitofrontal cortex and temporal polar region (some research did not even mention this component, see [30]) [28]. However, the limbic system (defined by anatomy/histology) and the midline/default-mode system (defined by rs-fMRI) overlapped substantially, nicely agreeing with the speculation of a recent study [39]. Despite the consistency, it is noteworthy that there is individual variability in terms of the spatial distribution of the constituent modules, and there are interesting individual-specific network patterns; for example, part of the precuneus may unite with the cuneus, the somatosensory module may correlate with anterior portion of the cuneus or part of the anterior cingulate cortex, the default-mode network may be divided into two sub-networks, insula or cingulo-insular network may be isolated, and the attentional fronto-parietal network may correlate with the cuneus (beyond the scope of this study, and the details thus not shown), etc. The variation might reflect individual neuropsychological characteristics that manifest at the large-scale network level, an area largely unexplored in current rs-fMRI research.
Given the importance of the FP topic in neuroimaging research, diverse analytic strategies have been developed and adopted to segment the cerebral cortex. It is appropriate to contrast our results with previous contributions to make the distinction of MOSI clear. There are three major categories of FP, briefly summarized below. The first class is the ROI-based approach that focuses on a particular structure. An ROI may be derived from the contrast of functional image analysis or, in an extreme case, may be reduced to a single voxel derived from the maximum activation of group-level analysis [39]. The second class is an atlas-based approach that pre-defines the cortex into tens to hundreds of regions and generally covers the whole brain [61],[62]. However, there has been a concern that the FP (and other measures, such as modularity analysis) based on atlas information may be inaccurate because the correspondence among different methods to define a homogeneous “functional” region is quite limited [10],[11]. If the validity of the upstream stage is questionable, the subsequent inferences about network properties could be biased (see alternative interpretation by [6]). There has been harsh criticism on the validity of atlas-based definitions of neural nodes [12],[13], which urges a data-driven approach to substantiate automatic whole-brain FP. The third class of FP is based on the features of brain signals per se, which provide a better representation of the brain dynamics. Various algorithms have been developed to serve the purpose of automatic whole-brain FP, such as sampling the brain via a regularly spaced grid and using whole-brain correlation information to guide FP or resorting to the trajectories of transition (abrupt changes in CC patterns based on each voxel) to define the parcellation boundaries [31],[34]. Expectation-maximization framework, variational Bayes approach, clustering techniques, independent component analysis and principal component analysis have all been incorporated into diverse FP algorithms under several experimental conditions [28],[49]–[55]. MOSI is different from all the above orientations in that local and neighboring homogeneity is the only pertinent consideration and constraint. Rather than extracting the “edge” that represents the boundary, MOSI respects the original definition of a module—the intensely connected members (voxels) are grouped together. After decomposing a larger module into several more homogeneous sub-modules, the neighboring sub-modules showing high similarity are united to form a new module. The splitting and unifying processes are repeated iteratively until the mutual information between the partitions of two successive cycles does not change. MOSI works fast, obeys the fundamental rationale to achieve data reduction (i.e., similar voxels are compressed to a node) and may lay the foundation for further individual network analysis. For example, the outputs of MOSI may provide novel starting points for multi-resolution structural network construction (e.g., the ROIs across different scales for diffusion tensor imaging analyses). The derived multi-resolution networks are innately multi-layered and may enable the exploration of the cross-scale network relationship. At each resolution, the output of MOSI may also endorse the study of scale-specific, multi-level network.
There are many clustering and community detection algorithms for FP of the brain but considering each of these algorithms is beyond the scope of this study. Nevertheless, it is well-known that different community detection algorithms may yield discrepant results (note: may also contribute to the individual variation in the network distribution described above) [6],[38],[63]–[65], which motivated Thirion et al. to examine the performance of different clustering algorithms for task-based fMRI datasets [66]. MOSI may provide a fair platform to enable the comparison of different community detection algorithms and to select the optimal algorithm/parameters tailored for resting-state brain network description. Other potential applications of MOSI include investigating the issue of hemispheric asymmetry [67], clarifying the debate on structural and functional concordance [45], characterizing network/graph properties, etc. It is also of interest to apply MOSI in neuropsychiatric populations and in the study of neural reorganization/plasticity. The authors expect that using MOSI as a data-reduction (pre-processing) procedure may enhance the consistency and comparability of network research in the field of rs-fMRI research.
A major challenge for FP of whole-brain fMRI data lies in the large voxel number; therefore, data-reduction strategies are frequently applied, such as regularly spaced seeds and atlas-defined ROIs [4],[34]. In contrast to previously proposed algorithms [31],[34], MOSI utilized the concordance in BOLD dynamics of the regional and neighboring voxels to parcellate the functional neuroimaging data and to achieve data reduction. By titrating gamma values, MOSI may also adapt the partition to different resolutions. The validity was confirmed by intra- and inter-individual analyses. The empirical analysis suggested that gamma values of 0.80–0.85 seem to be suitable for FP of the cortex to commonly reported networks. MOSI may provide a novel starting point for subsequent network analysis of rs-fMRI data.
[1] |
Baird G, Simonoff E, Pickles A, et al. (2006) Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet 368: 210-215. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69041-7
![]() |
[2] |
Baxter AJ, Brugha TS, Erskine HE, et al. (2015) The epidemiology and global burden of autism spectrum disorders. Psychol Med 45: 601-613. doi: 10.1017/S003329171400172X
![]() |
[3] |
Brugha TS, Spiers N, Bankart J, et al. (2016) Epidemiology of autism in adults across age groups and ability levels. Br J Psychiatry 209: 498-503. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.174649
![]() |
[4] |
Elsabbagh M, Divan G, Koh Y-J, et al. (2012) Global prevalence of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Autism Res 5: 160-179. doi: 10.1002/aur.239
![]() |
[5] |
Atladottir HO, Gyllenberg D, Langridge A, et al. (2015) The increasing prevalence of reported diagnoses of childhood psychiatric disorders: a descriptive multinational comparison. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 24: 173-183. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0553-8
![]() |
[6] |
Hansen SN, Schendel DE, Parner ET (2015) Explaining the increase in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders. The proportion attributable to changes in reporting practices. JAMA Pediatr 169: 56-62. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.1893
![]() |
[7] |
Simonoff E (2012) Autism spectrum disorder: prevalence and cause may be bound together. Br J Psych 201: 88-89. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.104703
![]() |
[8] |
Gillberg C (2010) The ESSENCE in child psychiatry: Early Symptomatic Syndromes Eliciting Neurodevelopmental Clinical Examinations. Res Dev Dis 31: 1543-1551. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.06.002
![]() |
[9] |
Grove J, Ripke S, Als TD, et al. (2019) Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat Genet 51: 431-444. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8
![]() |
[10] |
Simonoff E, Pickles A, Charman, et al. (2008) Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 47: 921-929. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f
![]() |
[11] |
Wender CLA, Veenstra-VanderWeele J (2017) Challenge and Potential for Research on Gene-Environment Interactions in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Gene-Environment Transactions in Developmental Psychopathology, Advances in Development and Psychopathology: Brain Research Foundation Symposium Series 2 Springer International Publishing AG, 157-176. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-49227-8_9
![]() |
[12] |
Sandin S, Lichtenstein P, Kuja-Halkola R, et al. (2014) The familial risk of autism. JAMA 311: 1770-1777. doi: 10.1001/jama.2014.4144
![]() |
[13] |
Robinson EB, St Pourcain B, Anttila V, et al. (2016) Genetic risk for autism spectrum disorders and neuropsychiatric variation in the general population. Nat Genet 48: 552-555. doi: 10.1038/ng.3529
![]() |
[14] |
Craddock N, Owen M (2010) The Kraepelinian dichotomy – going, going…but still not gone. Br J Psychiatry 196: 92-95. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.109.073429
![]() |
[15] | Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (2013) Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet 381: 1371-1379. |
[16] |
Aggernæs B (2018) Autism: A transdiagnostic, dimensional construct of reasoning? Eur J Neurosci 47: 515-533. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13599
![]() |
[17] | Aggernæs B (2016) Rethinking the concept of psychosis and the link between autism and Schizophrenia. Scand J Child Adol Psychiat Psychol 4: 4-11. |
[18] | Bleuler E (1911) Dementia Praecox oder Gruppe der Schizophrenien Leipzig: Deuticke. |
[19] | Piaget J (1967) La psychologie de l'intelligence Paris: A. Colin. |
[20] | Alberoni F (1984) Movement and Institution New York: Columbia University Press. |
[21] |
Aggernæs A, Haugsted R (1976) Experienced reality in three to six year old children: A study of direct reality testing. J Child Psychol Psychiat 17: 323-335. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976.tb00407.x
![]() |
[22] |
Aggernæs A, Paikin H, Vitger J (1981) Experienced reality in schizophrenia: How diffuse is the defect in reality testing? Indian J Psychol Med 4: 1-13. doi: 10.1177/0975156419810101
![]() |
[23] |
Clemmensen L, van Os J, Skovgaard AM, et al. (2014) Hyper-Theory-of-Mind in children with psychotic experiences. PLoS ONE 9: e113082. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113082
![]() |
[24] |
Hallmayer J, Cleveland S, Torres A, et al. (2011) Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68: 1095-1102. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.76
![]() |
[25] |
Latham KE, Sapienza C, Engel N (2012) The epigenetic lorax: gene–environment interactions in human health. Epigenomics 4: 383-402. doi: 10.2217/epi.12.31
![]() |
[26] |
Miake K, Hirasawa T, Koide T, et al. (2012) Epigenetics in autism and other neurodevelopmental diseases. Adv Exp Med Biol 724: 91-98. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-0653-2_7
![]() |
[27] |
Shanen NC (2006) Epigenetics of autism spectrum disorders. Human Molecular Genetics 15: R138-R150. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddl213
![]() |
[28] |
Siniscalco D, Cirillo A, Bradstreet JJ, et al. (2013) Epigenetic findings in autism: New perspectives for therapy. Int J Environ Res Public Health 10: 4261-4273. doi: 10.3390/ijerph10094261
![]() |
[29] |
Belmonte MK, Cook EH, Anderson GM, et al. (2004) Autism as a disorder of neural information processing: directions for research and targets for therapy. Mol Psychiatry 9: 646-663. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001499
![]() |
[30] |
Courchesne E (2002) Abnormal early brain development in autism. Mol Psychiatry 7: S21-S23. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001169
![]() |
[31] |
Menon V (2011) Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn Sci 15: 483-506. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003
![]() |
[32] |
Szatmari P (2000) The classification of autism, Asperger's syndrome, and pervasive developmental disorder. Can J Psychiat 45: 731-738. doi: 10.1177/070674370004500806
![]() |
[33] |
Szyf M (2009) Epigenetics, DNA methylation, and chromatin modifying drugs. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 49: 243-263. doi: 10.1146/annurev-pharmtox-061008-103102
![]() |
[34] |
Plomin R (2011) Commentary: Why are children in the same family so different? Non-shared environment three decades later. Int J Epidemiol 40: 582-592. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq144
![]() |
[35] |
Larsson HJ, Eaton WW, Madsen KM, et al. (2005) Risk factors for autism: perinatal factors, parental psychiatric history, and socioeconomic status. Am J Epidemiol 161: 916-925. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwi123
![]() |
[36] |
Lauritsen MB, Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB (2005) Effects of familial risk factors and place of birth on the risk of autism: a nationwide registerbased study. J Child Psychol Psyc 46: 963-971. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00391.x
![]() |
[37] |
Gardener H, Spiegelman D, Buka SL (2009) Prenatal risk factors for autism: comprehensive meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 195: 7-14. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051672
![]() |
[38] | Ramaswami G, Geschwind DH (2018) Chapter 21 Genetics of autism spectrum disorder. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 147 (3rd series) Neurogenetics, Part I Elsevier, 321-329. |
[39] |
Betancur C (2011) Etiological heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: More than 100 genetic and genomic disorders and still counting. Brain Res 1380: 42-77. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.078
![]() |
[40] |
Nava C, Keren B, Mignot C, et al. (2014) Prospective diagnostic analysis of copy number variants using SNP microarrays in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Eur J Hum Genet 22: 71-78. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2013.88
![]() |
[41] | The Autism Genome Project Consortium (2007) Mapping autism risk loci using genetic linkage and chromosomal rearrangements. Nat Genet 39: 319-328. |
[42] |
Weiner DJ, Wigdor EM, Ripke S, et al. (2017) Polygenic transmission disequilibrium confirms that common and rare variation act additively to create risk for autism spectrum disorders. Nat Genet 49: 978-985. doi: 10.1038/ng.3863
![]() |
[43] |
Gaugler T, Klei L, Sanders SJ, et al. (2014) Most genetic risk for autism resides with common variation. Nat Genet 46: 881-885. doi: 10.1038/ng.3039
![]() |
[44] |
Guilmatre A, Dubourg C, Mosca AL, et al. (2009) Recurrent rearrangements in synaptic and neurodevelopmental genes and shared biologic pathways in schizophrenia, autism, and mental retardation. Arch Gen Psychiat 66: 947-956. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.80
![]() |
[45] |
McCarthy SE, Gillis J, Kramer M, et al. (2014) De novo mutations in schizophrenia implicate chromatin remodeling and support a genetic overlap with autism and intellectual disability. Mol Psychiatry 19: 652-658. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.29
![]() |
[46] |
Hannon E, Schendel D, Ladd-Acosta C, et al. (2018) Elevated polygenic burden for autism is associated with differential DNA methylation at birth. Genome Med 10: 19. doi: 10.1186/s13073-018-0527-4
![]() |
[47] | Tran NQV, Miyake K (2017) Neurodevelopmental disorders and environmental toxicants: epigenetics as an underlying mechanism. Int J Genomics 2017: 7526592. |
[48] |
Waddington CH (2012) The epigenotype. Int J Epidemiol 41: 10-13. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr184
![]() |
[49] |
Fraga MF, Ballestar E, Paz MF, et al. (2005) Epigenetic differences arise during the lifetime of monozygotic twins. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 10604-10609. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500398102
![]() |
[50] |
Lee R, Avramopoulos D (2014) Introduction to Epigenetics in Psychiatry. Epigenetics in Psychiatry Elsevier Inc., 3-25. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-417114-5.00001-2
![]() |
[51] |
Bale TL (2014) Lifetime stress experience: transgenerational epigenetics and germ cell programming. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 16: 297-305. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2014.16.3/tbale
![]() |
[52] | Gos M (2013) Epigenetic mechanisms of gene expression regulation in neurological diseases. Acta Neurobiol Exp 73: 19-37. |
[53] |
James SJ, Melnyk S, Jernigan S, et al. (2006) Metabolic endophenotype and related genotypes are associated with oxidative stress in children with autism. Am J Med Genet Part B 141B: 947-956. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30366
![]() |
[54] |
Deth R, Muratore C, Benzecry J, et al. (2008) How environmental and genetic factors combine to cause autism: A redox/methylation hypothesis. Neurotoxicology 29: 190-201. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2007.09.010
![]() |
[55] |
Homs A, Codina-Solà M, Rodríguez-Santiago B, et al. (2016) Genetic and epigenetic methylation defects and implication of the ERMN gene in autism spectrum disorders. Transl Psychiatry 6: e855. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.120
![]() |
[56] |
Ginsberg MR, Rubin RA, Falcone T, et al. (2012) Brain transcriptional and epigenetic associations with autism. PLoS ONE 7: e44736. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044736
![]() |
[57] |
Wong CCY, Meaburn EL, Ronald A, et al. (2014) Methylomic analysis of monozygotic twins discordant for autism spectrum disorder and related behavioural traits. Mol Psychiatry 19: 495-503. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.41
![]() |
[58] |
Minnis H (2013) Maltreatment-Associated Psychiatric Problems: An Example of Environmentally Triggered ESSENCE? Sci World J 2013: 148468. doi: 10.1155/2013/148468
![]() |
[59] | Pritchett R, Pritchett J, Marshall E, et al. (2013) Reactive attachment disorder in the general population: a hidden ESSENCE disorder. Sci World J 2013: 81815. |
[60] |
Dinkler L, Lundström S, Gajwani R, et al. (2017) Maltreatment-associated neurodevelopmental disorders: a co-twin control analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 58: 691-701. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12682
![]() |
[61] |
McEwen BS (2008) Central effects of stress hormones in health and disease: understanding the protective and damaging effects of stress and stress mediators. Eur J Pharmacol 583: 174-185. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.11.071
![]() |
[62] | Rostène W, Sarrieau A, Nicot A, et al. (1995) Steroid effects on brain functions: An example of the action of glucocorticoids on central dopaminergic and neurotensinergic systems. J Psychiatry Neurosci 20: 349-356. |
[63] |
Meaney MJ, Szyf M (2005) Environmental programming of stress responses through DNA methylation: life at the interface between a dynamic environment and a fixed genome. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 7: 103-123. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2005.7.2/mmeaney
![]() |
[64] |
Weaver ICG, Champagne FA, Brown SE, et al. (2005) Reversal of maternal programming of stress responses in adult offspring through methyl supplementation: Altering epigenetic marking later in life. J Neurosci 25: 11045-11054. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3652-05.2005
![]() |
[65] |
McGowan PO, Matthews SG (2018) Prenatal stress, glucocorticoids, and developmental programming of the stress response. Endocrinology 159: 69-82. doi: 10.1210/en.2017-00896
![]() |
[66] |
Stenz L, Schechter DS, Serpa SR, et al. (2018) Intergenerational transmission of DNA methylation signatures associated with early life stress. Curr Genomics 19: 665-675. doi: 10.2174/1389202919666171229145656
![]() |
[67] |
Oberlander TF, Weinberg J, Papsdorf M, et al. (2008) Prenatal exposure to maternal depression, neonatal methylation of human glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1) and infant cortisol stress responses. Epigenetics 3: 97-106. doi: 10.4161/epi.3.2.6034
![]() |
[68] | Corbett BA, Mendoza S, Wegelin JA, et al. (2008) Variable cortisol circadian rhythms in children with autism and anticipatory stress. J Psychiatry Neurosci 33: 227-234. |
[69] |
Corbett BA, Schupp CW, Levine S, et al. (2009) Comparing cortisol, stress, and sensory sensitivity in children with autism. Autism Res 2: 39-49. doi: 10.1002/aur.64
![]() |
[70] |
Corbett BA, Schupp CW, Lanni KE (2012) Comparing biobehavioral profiles across two social stress paradigms in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Mol Autism 3: 13. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-3-13
![]() |
[71] |
Corbett BA, Muscatello RA, Blain SD (2016) Impact of sensory sensitivity on physiological stress response and novel peer interaction in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Front Neurosci 10: 278. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00278
![]() |
[72] |
Tordjman S, Anderson GM, Kermarrec S, et al. (2014) Altered circadian patterns of salivary cortisol in low-functioning children and adolescents with autism. Psychoneuroendocrinology 50: 227-245. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.010
![]() |
[73] |
Bishop-Fitzpatrick L, Mazefsky CA, Minshew NJ, et al. (2015) The relationship between stress and social functioning in adults with autism spectrum disorder and without intellectual disability. Autism Res 8: 164-173. doi: 10.1002/aur.1433
![]() |
[74] |
Bishop-Fitzpatrick L, Minshew NJ, Mazefsky CA, et al. (2017) Perception of life as stressful, not biological response to stress, is associated with greater social disability in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Dev Disord 47: 1-16. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2910-6
![]() |
[75] |
Sønderby IE, Gústafsson Ó, Doan NT, et al. (2018) Dose response of the 16p11.2 distal copy number variant on intracranial volume and basal ganglia. Mol Psychiatry 25: 584-602. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0118-1
![]() |
[76] |
Turner TN, Coe BP, Dickel DE, et al. (2017) Genomic patterns of de novo mutation in simplex autism. Cell 171: 710-722. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.08.047
![]() |
[77] |
Schork AJ, Won H, Appadurai V, et al. (2019) A genome-wide association study of shared risk across psychiatric disorders implicates gene regulation during fetal neurodevelopment. Nat Neurosci 22: 353-361. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0320-0
![]() |
[78] |
Gandal MJ, Haney JR, Parikshak NN, et al. (2018) Shared molecular neuropathology across major psychiatric disorders parallels polygenic overlap. Science 359: 693-697. doi: 10.1126/science.aad6469
![]() |
[79] |
Shulha HP, Cheung I, Guo Y, et al. (2013) Coordinated cell type–specific epigenetic remodeling in prefrontal cortex begins before birth and continues into early adulthood. PLoS Genet 9: e1003433. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003433
![]() |
[80] |
Belmonte MK, Yurgelun-Todd DA (2003) Functional anatomy of impaired selective attention and compensatory processing in autism. Brain Res Cogn Res 17: 651-664. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00189-7
![]() |
[81] |
Livingston LA, Happé F (2017) Conceptualising compensation in neurodevelopmental disorders: Reflections from autism spectrum disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 80: 729-742. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.06.005
![]() |
[82] |
Weems CF (2015) Biological correlates of child and adolescent responses to disaster exposure: A bio-ecological model. Curr Psychiatry Rep 17: 1-7. doi: 10.1007/s11920-015-0588-7
![]() |
[83] |
Weems CF, Russel JD, Neill EL, et al. (2019) Annual Research Review: Pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder from a neurodevelopmental network perspective. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 60: 395-408. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12996
![]() |
[84] | Ouss-Ryngaert L, Golse B (2010) Linking neuroscience and psychoanalysis from a developmental perspective: Why and how? J Physiol 104: 303-308. |
[85] |
Hosman CMH, van Doesum KTM, van Santvoort F (2009) Prevention of emotional problems and psychiatric risks in children of parents with a mental illness in the Netherlands: I. The scientific basis to a comprehensive approach. Aust e-J Advancement Ment Health 8: 250-263. doi: 10.5172/jamh.8.3.250
![]() |
[86] | Sarovic D (2018) A framework for neurodevelopmental disorders: Operationalization of a pathogenetic triad for clinical and research use. Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Sahlgrenska Academy University of Gothenburg Available from: https://psyarxiv.com/mbeqh/. |
[87] |
Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, et al. (2014) The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clin Psychol Sci 2: 119-137. doi: 10.1177/2167702613497473
![]() |
[88] |
Sameroff AJ, Mackenzie MJ (2003) Research strategies for capturing transactional models of development: The limits of the possible. Dev Psychopathol 15: 613-640. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000312
![]() |
[89] |
Bishop-Fitzpatrick L, Mazefsky CA, Eack SM (2018) The combined impact of social support and perceived stress on quality of life in adults with autism spectrum disorder and without intellectual disability. Autism 22: 703-711. doi: 10.1177/1362361317703090
![]() |
[90] |
Bishop-Fitzpatrick L, Smith LE, Greenberg JS, et al. (2017) Participation in recreational activities buffers the impact of perceived stress on quality of life in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res 10: 973-982. doi: 10.1002/aur.1753
![]() |
[91] |
Yehuda R, Lehrner A, Bierer LM (2018) The public reception of putative epigenetic mechanisms in the transgenerational effects of trauma. Environ Epigenet 4: 1-7. doi: 10.1093/eep/dvy018
![]() |
[92] |
Clarke T-K, Lupton MK, Fernandez-Pujals AM, et al. (2016) Common polygenic risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is associated with cognitive ability in the general population. Mol Psychiatry 21: 419-425. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.12
![]() |
[93] |
Robinson EB, Samocha KE, Kosmicki JA, et al. (2014) Autism spectrum disorder severity reflects the average contribution of de novo and familial influences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111: 15161-15165. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1409204111
![]() |
[94] |
Sanders SJ, He X, Willsey AJ, et al. (2015) Insights into autism spectrum disorder genomic architecture and biology from 71 risk loci. Neuron 87: 1215-1233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.016
![]() |
[95] |
Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH (2006) Gene-environment interaction of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) and observed maternal insensitivity predicting externalizing behavior in preschoolers. Dev Psychobiol 48: 406-409. doi: 10.1002/dev.20152
![]() |
[96] |
Kumsta R, Stevens S, Brookes K, et al. (2010) 5HTT genotype moderates the influence of early institutional deprivation on emotional problems in adolescence: evidence from the English and Romanian Adoptee (ERA) study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 51: 755-762. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02249.x
![]() |
[97] |
Yang C-J, Tan H-P, Yang F-Y (2015) The cortisol, serotonin and oxytocin are associated with repetitive behavior in autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord 18: 12-20. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2015.07.002
![]() |
[98] |
Meier SM, Petersen L, Schendel DE, et al. (2015) Obsessive-compulsive disorder and autism spectrum disorders: Longitudinal and offspring risk. PLoS ONE 10: e0141703. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141703
![]() |
[99] |
Valla JM, Belmonte MK (2013) Detail-oriented cognitive style and social communicative deficits, within and beyond the autism spectrum: independent traits that grow into developmental interdependence. Dev Rev 33: 371-398. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2013.08.004
![]() |
[100] |
Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Ruigrok AN, et al. (2017) Quantifying and exploring camouflaging in men and women with autism. Autism 21: 690-702. doi: 10.1177/1362361316671012
![]() |
[101] |
Atladóttir HO, Thorsen P, Schendel DE, et al. (2010) Association of hospitalization for infection in childhood with diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 164: 470-477. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.9
![]() |
[102] |
Nudel R, Wang Y, Appadurai V, et al. (2019) A large-scale genomic investigation of susceptibility to infection and its association with mental disorders in the Danish population. Transl Psychiatry 9: 283. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0622-3
![]() |
[103] |
Danese A, Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, et al. (2017) The origins of cognitive deficits in victimized children: Implications for neuroscientists and clinicians. Am J Psychiatry 174: 349-361. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16030333
![]() |
[104] |
Kim SY, Bottema-Beutel K (2019) A meta regression analysis of quality of life correlates in adults with ASD. Res Autism Spectr Disord 63: 23-33. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2018.11.004
![]() |
[105] |
Plana-Ripoll O, Pedersen CB, Holtz Y, et al. (2019) Exploring comorbidity within mental disorders among a danish national population. JAMA Psychiatry 76: 259-270. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.3658
![]() |
[106] |
Cuthbert N, Insel R (2013) Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med 11: 126. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-11-126
![]() |
[107] | (2013) American Psychiatric AssociationDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. American Psychiatric Publishing. |
[108] | (1993) WHOThe ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: World Health Organization. |
[109] |
Polanczyk GV, Casella C, Jaffee SR (2019) Commentary: ADHD lifetime trajectories and the relevance of the developmental perspective to Psychiatry: reflections on Asherson and Agnew-Blais. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 60: 353-355. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13050
![]() |
[110] |
Narusyte J, Neiderhiser JM, D'Onofrio BM, et al. (2008) Testing different types of genotype–environment correlation: An extended children-of-twins model. Dev Psychol 44: 1591-1603. doi: 10.1037/a0013911
![]() |
[111] |
Sykes NH, Lamb JA (2007) Autism: The quest for the genes. Expert Rev Mol Med 9: 1-15. doi: 10.1017/S1462399407000452
![]() |
1. | Tien-Wen Lee, Gerald Tramontano, Clay Hinrichs, Concordant dynamic changes of global network properties in the frontoparietal and limbic compartments: An EEG study, 2024, 235, 03032647, 105101, 10.1016/j.biosystems.2023.105101 | |
2. | X Yi, ZM Wang, X Heng, Intention Recognition Method Based on Resting-state and P300 Task-state Dynamic Brain Functional Network Features, 2023, 2476, 1742-6588, 012052, 10.1088/1742-6596/2476/1/012052 | |
3. | Tien-Wen Lee, A novel form of dialectics informed by psychiatry and brain science: centralized dialectics, 2025, 1046-1310, 10.1007/s12144-025-07481-z | |
4. | Tien-Wen Lee, Gerald Tramontano, Negative relationship between inter-regional interaction and regional power: insights from separate resting fMRI and EEG datasets, 2025, 01650270, 110438, 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2025.110438 | |
5. | Tien-Wen Lee, Framing major depressive disorder as a condition of network imbalance at the compartment level: a proof-of-concept study, 2025, 35, 1047-3211, 10.1093/cercor/bhaf089 |
gamma | mean | s.d. | number (L/R) | time (sec) |
original | 0.33 | 0.034 | 25/25 | n/a |
0.65 | 0.40 | 0.036 | 20.2/19.5 | 124.0 |
0.70 | 0.41 | 0.040 | 23.2/23.4 | 199.2 |
0.75 | 0.41 | 0.036 | 26.9/26.9 | 317.8 |
0.80 | 0.43 | 0.026 | 38.6/37.6 | 337.7 |
0.85 | 0.47 | 0.022 | 55.3/54.2 | 359.5 |
0.90 | 0.53 | 0.018 | 91.7/91.4 | 511.7 |
0.95 | 0.60 | 0.015 | 169.3/169.3 | 1542.5 |
gamma | original | 0.65 | 0.70 | 0.75 | 0.80 | 0.85 | 0.90 | 0.95 |
original | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 0.13 | 0.19 | 0.26 | |
0.65 | 12.8 | 0.01* | 0.01* | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.14 | 0.21 | |
0.70 | 15.6 | 2.1* | 0.00* | 0.02 | 0.06 | 0.12 | 0.19 | |
0.75 | 17.4 | 2.8* | 0.9* | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.11 | 0.17 | |
0.80 | 25.0 | 6.8 | 5.3 | 5.9 | 0.04 | 0.10 | 0.13 | |
0.85 | 31.4 | 13.7 | 12.3 | 14.5 | 14.8 | 0.06 | 0.07 | |
0.90 | 34.9 | 23.8 | 20.0 | 21.3 | 25.3 | 18.9 | ||
0.95 | 47.8 | 36.2 | 31.0 | 33.7 | 43.6 | 41.9 | 31.0 |
Note: the sign of the number is ignored because the grand mean CC for higher gamma values is always higher than that of the lower gamma values. For all the pair-wise comparisons, the
gamma | VA | DM | SM | FP | LP |
0.65 | 83 | 79 | 87 | 80 | 62 |
0.70 | 88 | 85 | 90 | 78 | 82 |
0.75 | 89 | 89 | 99 | 77 | 88 |
0.80 | 85 | 95 | 87 | 83 | 86 |
0.85 | 87 | 95 | 98 | 83 | 81 |
0.90 | 91 | 97 | 94 | 96 | 69 |
0.95 | 86 | 96 | 94 | 95 | 64 |
gamma | mean | s.d. | number (L/R) | time (sec) |
original | 0.33 | 0.034 | 25/25 | n/a |
0.65 | 0.40 | 0.036 | 20.2/19.5 | 124.0 |
0.70 | 0.41 | 0.040 | 23.2/23.4 | 199.2 |
0.75 | 0.41 | 0.036 | 26.9/26.9 | 317.8 |
0.80 | 0.43 | 0.026 | 38.6/37.6 | 337.7 |
0.85 | 0.47 | 0.022 | 55.3/54.2 | 359.5 |
0.90 | 0.53 | 0.018 | 91.7/91.4 | 511.7 |
0.95 | 0.60 | 0.015 | 169.3/169.3 | 1542.5 |
gamma | original | 0.65 | 0.70 | 0.75 | 0.80 | 0.85 | 0.90 | 0.95 |
original | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 0.13 | 0.19 | 0.26 | |
0.65 | 12.8 | 0.01* | 0.01* | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.14 | 0.21 | |
0.70 | 15.6 | 2.1* | 0.00* | 0.02 | 0.06 | 0.12 | 0.19 | |
0.75 | 17.4 | 2.8* | 0.9* | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.11 | 0.17 | |
0.80 | 25.0 | 6.8 | 5.3 | 5.9 | 0.04 | 0.10 | 0.13 | |
0.85 | 31.4 | 13.7 | 12.3 | 14.5 | 14.8 | 0.06 | 0.07 | |
0.90 | 34.9 | 23.8 | 20.0 | 21.3 | 25.3 | 18.9 | ||
0.95 | 47.8 | 36.2 | 31.0 | 33.7 | 43.6 | 41.9 | 31.0 |
gamma | VA | DM | SM | FP | LP |
0.65 | 83 | 79 | 87 | 80 | 62 |
0.70 | 88 | 85 | 90 | 78 | 82 |
0.75 | 89 | 89 | 99 | 77 | 88 |
0.80 | 85 | 95 | 87 | 83 | 86 |
0.85 | 87 | 95 | 98 | 83 | 81 |
0.90 | 91 | 97 | 94 | 96 | 69 |
0.95 | 86 | 96 | 94 | 95 | 64 |