Citation: Matthew S. Sherwood, Jason G. Parker, Emily E. Diller, Subhashini Ganapathy, Kevin Bennett, Jeremy T. Nelson. Volitional down-regulation of the primary auditory cortex via directed attention mediated by real-time fMRI neurofeedback[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2018, 5(3): 179-199. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.3.179
[1] | Byron Bernal, Alfredo Ardila . From Hearing Sounds to Recognizing Phonemes: Primary Auditory Cortex is A Truly Perceptual Language Area. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(4): 454-473. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.4.454 |
[2] | Tien-Wen Lee, Gerald Tramontano . Automatic parcellation of resting-state cortical dynamics by iterative community detection and similarity measurements. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(4): 526-542. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021028 |
[3] | Aini Ismafairus Abd Hamid, Nurfaten Hamzah, Siti Mariam Roslan, Nur Alia Amalin Suhardi, Muhammad Riddha Abdul Rahman, Faiz Mustafar, Hazim Omar, Asma Hayati Ahmad, Elza Azri Othman, Ahmad Nazlim Yusoff . Distinct neural mechanisms of alpha binaural beats and white noise for cognitive enhancement in young adults. AIMS Neuroscience, 2025, 12(2): 147-179. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2025010 |
[4] | Zoha Deldar, Carlos Gevers-Montoro, Ali Khatibi, Ladan Ghazi-Saidi . The interaction between language and working memory: a systematic review of fMRI studies in the past two decades. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(1): 1-32. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021001 |
[5] | Mirela Dubravac, Beat Meier . Stimulating the parietal cortex by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): no effects on attention and memory. AIMS Neuroscience, 2021, 8(1): 33-46. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2021002 |
[6] | Charles R Larson, Donald A Robin . Sensory Processing: Advances in Understanding Structure and Function of Pitch-Shifted Auditory Feedback in Voice Control. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(1): 22-39. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.1.22 |
[7] | Evangelos Almpanis, Constantinos Siettos . Construction of functional brain connectivity networks from fMRI data with driving and modulatory inputs: an extended conditional Granger causality approach. AIMS Neuroscience, 2020, 7(2): 66-88. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2020005 |
[8] | Byron Bernal, Alfredo Ardila, Monica Rosselli . The Network of Brodmanns Area 22 in Lexico-semantic Processing: A Pooling-data Connectivity Study. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(3): 306-316. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.3.306 |
[9] | Graziella Orrù, Marina Baroni, Ciro Conversano, Angelo Gemignani . Exploring the therapeutic potential of tDCS, TMS and DBS in overcoming tobacco use disorder: an umbrella review. AIMS Neuroscience, 2024, 11(4): 449-467. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2024027 |
[10] | Elisa Magosso, Giulia Ricci, Mauro Ursino . Modulation of brain alpha rhythm and heart rate variability by attention-related mechanisms. AIMS Neuroscience, 2019, 6(1): 1-24. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2019.1.1 |
Advances in acquisition techniques, computational power, and algorithms have revolutionized the speed in which functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) data can be measured and processed. This acceleration has led to real-time fMRI, where fMRI data (i.e., blood-oxygen-level-dependent [BOLD] signals) can be processed faster than it is collected. There are currently four domains where real-time fMRI is being implemented: Intraoperative surgical guidance [1], brain-computer interfaces [2,3], adapting stimuli for current brain states [4], and neurofeedback training [5].
Neurofeedback training (NFT), although not the original focus of real-time fMRI, is a growing field of research where BOLD signals are presented using visual or auditory stimuli during data acquisition so the subject may learn to modulate the signals at will (i.e., closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation). This technique differs from traditional fMRI where individuals respond to exogenous stimuli without being informed of the timing and location of induced brain activity (i.e., open-loop neuromodulation), as well as exogenous neuromodulation techniques like transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) or pharmacotherapy. Information regarding the activity of a specific brain region is presented to the subject in real-time during fMRI-NFT. Through the implementation of mental strategies, individuals learn to self-regulate the BOLD signal and, therefore, brain activity as these are tightly coupled through neurovascular mechanisms [6,7,8]. Researchers have shown that people can learn volitional control over the BOLD signal measured from numerous brain regions including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) [9], amygdala [10], anterior insula [11,12], auditory and attention related networks [13], bilateral rostrolateral prefrontal cortex [14], left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [15,16,17], motor cortices [18,19,20], primary auditory cortex [21,22,23], regions associated with emotional network [24,25], right inferior frontal gyrus [26], and visual cortices [27,28].
The efficacy of fMRI-NFT in altering behavior was demonstrated for the first time in 2015 [29]. Since, fMRI-NFT has been demonstrated across a broad range of medical applications. In one study, participants suffering from schizophrenia were able to learn control over the BOLD signal measured from the left and right insula [30]. In another study, an experimental group of participants diagnosed with Parkinson's disease exhibited significant clinical and functional improvements which were not observed from a control group of patients who received sham feedback [31]. Other studies have demonstrated potential applications for people suffering from major depression [32] and chronic tinnitus [21]. One group of researchers combined fMRI-NFT with TMS and found that endogenous neuromodulation of the ventral premotor cortex helps decrease intracortical inhibition measured from TMS. This application may enhance facilitation of stroke victims [18]. Overall, this trend represents the wide range of medical applications of fMRI-NFT but also the specificity of training required for each application.
In several previous studies, control groups who received sham BOLD signals lacked the differences in activity observed from those who received true feedback [9,10,11,13,14,20,26,27,29,31,33,34], implicating neuromodulation or behavioral training strategies affecting global arousal are not effective. Additionally, control groups which received identical instructions and the same period of training but no feedback on the current level of brain activity did not exhibit similar results as the experimental groups who were given neurofeedback [11,14,24,29,32]. These findings suggest the experiential effects are attributable to real-time fMRI-induced learning rather than other learning or nonspecific changes. Therefore, specific training regimens must be developed which target specific neurophysiological systems to obtain the desired effects. The results from the control groups in Decharms et al. [29] further indicate behavioral training, practice, sensory feedback, and biofeedback alone do not produce effects that are equivalent to those obtained with control groups who receive NFT from real-time fMRI. The exact mechanism translating neuromodulation into behavioral effects are still unknown. One postulation is the brain network responsible for the behavior is reinforced when one actively regulates neural activity in one or more of these regions. Such reinforcement results in the engagement of neuroplastic mechanisms causing the network to execute more efficiently. This theory coincides with other neuromodulation training techniques such as EEG-based neurofeedback where individuals are trained to control frequency bands of electrical signals measured from the local regions of the scalp [35,36,37]. Another hypothesis is that participants who learn appropriate mental strategies to modulate the BOLD signal will recruit task related brain networks more readily than others when processing stimuli [16].
In this work, we investigated the use of fMRI-NFT to teach volitional down-regulation of A1 during binaural auditory stimulation using directed attention strategies. It is not currently known whether individuals are capable of down-regulating A1 in the presence of auditory stimulation. However, two previous studies indicate that volitional down-regulation of A1 activation is achievable. In the one study, twenty-two healthy participants were divided into two equal groups completing the same tasks: one group received neurofeedback and the other group did not. All participants underwent multiple sessions consisting of five neurofeedback blocks. During neurofeedback, participants were asked to increase the activated volume in the primary and secondary auditory cortex from auditory stimulation. The change in activated volume was indicated at the end of each neurofeedback block only for the experimental group. This study found that participants receiving neurofeedback were successful in increasing the activated volume in the primary and secondary auditory cortex using fMRI-NFT; those not receiving neurofeedback were unsuccessful and did not exhibit any trends of habituation to the noise in the fMRI data [23]. In the other study, six participants with chronic tinnitus underwent 4 runs of fMRI-NFT conducted in a single session. It was reported that tinnitus patients were able to volitionally increase A1 activation. However, the study was not controlled so this finding could not be necessarily attributable to fMRI-NFT [21]. The second indicates that fMRI-NFT can train volitional down-regulation. In one uncontrolled study, schizophrenia patients were successfully able to learn volitional down-regulation of the superior temporal gyrus over four fMRI-NFT sessions [38]. In another controlled study, a group of 16 healthy females learned to down-regulate activation of the amygdala in the presence of aversive scenes using one fMRI-NFT session[39]. We used these findings to hypothesize that an experimental group receiving real neurofeedback will have greater volitional control over A1 de-activation than a control group receiving sham feedback.
Healthy volunteers were recruited from Wright State University and the surrounding community. Prior to being enrolled, each potential participant completed a telephone screening to qualify for the study. Forty-seven (47) participants meeting the inclusion/exclusion criteria were recruited (no contraindication to MRI procedures, between the ages of 18 and 50, right handed, no unstable medical or mental illness, no history of neurologic disorders, no hearing loss > 40 dB). These participants were selected at random from the qualifying group. The study was approved by Wright State University's Institutional Review Board (IRB) and the Air Force Medical Support Agency Surgeon General's Research Oversight Committee; informed consent was obtained prior to the execution of any experimental procedures. Participants eligible for compensation received equal remuneration.
Participants were randomly assigned to one of two groups and were blinded to the assigned group. The experimental group (EXP) received real feedback regarding activity in A1 during closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation. The control group (CON) was supplied with sham feedback yoked from a random participant in the experimental group matched for training time. Nineteen (19) participants voluntarily withdrew or were withdrawn from the study due to excessive motion, absenteeism/tardiness, or software/hardware issues limiting the completion of study procedures. The MRI data for a single participant was corrupted. This resulted in a final group of eighteen (18) EXP participants (mean age 23.2 +/− 1.1, 11 males) and nine (9) CON participants (mean age 24.4 +/− 2.5, 4 males).
All participants first completed a consent visit. After obtaining informed consent, the participants completed a MRI screening form. Next, a short hearing test was conducted to verify normal hearing (no frequency > 40 dB on a standard audiogram; Shoebox Audiometry, Ontario, Canada). This test is a simple self-applied test that has been clinically validated [40,41]. Following the consent visit, the subjects completed five fMRI-NFT sessions. All MRI procedures were conducted on a 3 Tesla (T) MRI (Discovery 750 W, GE Healthcare, Madison, WI) using a 24-channel head coil. These five sessions were executed within 21 days (EXP: 14.61 +/− 0.71 days; CON: 12.44 +/− 1.59) with only one per day.
We performed fMRI-NFT across five sessions for each participant. Prior to entering the MRI environment, MRI screening forms were reviewed by a registered MRI technician. Female participants were required to take a urine dipstick pregnancy test. Once entering the MRI, the participants first inserted MRI-compatible ear plugs (MagnaCoil, Magnacoustics Inc., Atlantic Beach, NY) capable of providing communication and auditory stimulation (Genesis Ultra, Magnacoustics Inc., Atlantic Beach, NY). Next, the participants were positioned supine on the MRI table, their head was padded to restrict motion, and the upper part of the 24-channel head coil was attached. Using a laser, the nasion was landmarked relative to the MRI. The landmarked position was moved to the center of the MRI bore.
Once positioned, the fMRI-NFT procedures began (Figure 1). Each fMRI-NFT session consisted of a single run of bilateral auditory stimulation which was used to individually and functionally localize A1. This scan is referred to as the “functional localizer”. Two runs of closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation followed the functional localizer. A structural MRI was performed between the functional localizer and closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation runs. The structural MRI was acquired using an 3D brain volume imaging (BRAVO) pulse sequence which acquires images using an inversion recovery prepared fast spoiled gradient-echo (FSPGR). The structural images were acquired using a 256 × 256 element matrix, 172 slices oriented in the same plane as the functional scans, 1 mm3 isotropic voxels, 0.8 phase field of view factor, TI/TE = 450/3.224 ms, a flip angle of 13°, and an auto-calibrated reconstruction for cartesian sampling with a phase acceleration factor of 2.0. The left and right A1 were manually identified using anatomical markers and an activation map produced from the functional localizer. Once identified, a region-of-interest (ROI) was selected from the voxels in the left and right A1 most robustly activated during the functional localizer. The BOLD signals from this ROI were used to generate the subsequent neurofeedback.
A single run of binaural auditory stimulation was executed to identify A1 using a boxcar design with eight (8) repetitions of OFF and ON blocks. The auditory stimuli were 10 kHz lowpass filtered white noise with a 6 dB roll-off and a 0.5 s fade-in (Audacity 2.1.3, www.audacity.org). The duration of each block was 20 s, and the first block began after the acquisition of four (4) dummy volumes and one (1) software preparation volume. Binaural auditory stimulation was delivered via the headphones only during ON blocks and controlled via a stimulus presentation software (Presentation, Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Berkeley, CA). Auditory stimulation consisted of 10 kHz lowpass filtered white noise presented at 90 dB, previously shown to be effective at producing a BOLD response [21]. The participants were not required to respond in any way during the scan, however they were instructed to remain awake and to focus on a round fixation dot presented in gray with a black background on a MRI-compatible display (SensaVue, Invivo, Gainesville, FL). FMRI data were acquired using a gradient-recalled-echo (GRE) sequence sensitive to the BOLD signal. This sequence acquired data using the following parameters: 64 × 64 element matrix, 41 slices oriented parallel to the AC-PC plane, 3.5 × 3.5 × 3 mm3 voxels size, 0.5 mm slice gap, TR/TE = 2000/20 ms, and a flip angle of 90° with fat suppression enabled. In previous data collections, these parameters have been shown to reduce susceptibility artifacts which can be significant at high field strengths such as 3T.
Immediately following acquisition, the BOLD data were pre-processed using custom MATLAB and C++ software. The pre-processing included standard spatial filtering (3D, 5-point Gaussian low-pass kernel, full-width half-maximum of 7 mm), motion correction (corrected to the first volume using a rigid-body 3-parameter model) and temporal filtering (5-point Gaussian low-pass kernel, sigma of 3 s) processing functions [42].
An activation map was created by defining a single explanatory variable (EV) by convolving a boxcar model containing 20 s control and task conditions with a pre-defined HRF [43]. Next, the BOLD data at each voxel was fit to the model using a general linear model (GLM) by applying a weight of +1 to the EV, representative of activation (positive correlation to the model). The resulting β parameter maps were converted to t statistic maps (activation maps) using standard statistical transforms. The region in A1 in which the feedback signal for the subsequent closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation runs was derived from this activation map. Voxels were added to the A1 ROI by first locating the axial slice in which the inferior surface of the anterior ventricle horns is visible. Finally, activation patterns on the left and right hemispheres near the posterior end of the lateral sulci were observed. Voxels within this region responding robustly to binaural auditory stimulation were added to the ROI to complete the determination of the functional localizer.
Two runs of closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation were completed following the functional localizer. BOLD data was acquired using the same scan parameters as described for the functional localizer. Four dummy volumes and one software preparation volume were acquired first. Then, eight volumes were acquired to determine a baseline BOLD signal value for the selected A1 ROI. During the acquisition of the baseline volumes, a countdown was displayed on the screen, however there was no auditory stimulation during either the eight baseline volumes or the five preparatory volumes. In the subsequent scanning for the experimental group, a feedback signal was computed and displayed to the participants from real-time analysis of BOLD data. This real-time analysis was implemented in custom MATLAB and C++ software, and included standard spatial filtering (3D, 5-point Gaussian low-pass kernel, full-width half-maximum of 7 mm) and motion-correction (corrected to the first volume of the functional localizer using a rigid-body 3-parameter model) processing functions [42]. This custom software further compared the average BOLD signal in the voxels selected from the functional localizer at baseline to that of the current volume to derive the percent signal change. The current feedback signal was determined by temporally-filtering (5-point Gaussian low-pass kernel consisting of only past components, sigma of 3 s) the percent BOLD signal change with the feedback signals from previous volumes. This feedback signal was presented to the participants using a thermometer-style bar plot within an average of 750 ms from the end of acquisition of a complete volume (∼500 ms for reconstruction and DICOM writing/reading, ∼250 ms for data processing and display). The thermometer plot contained a running average of the previous four values and a running task minimum. For participants in the control group, the feedback signal was yoked from a random EXP participant with experimental progress matched. Both runs from each session were duplicated from the same EXP participant but the EXP participant was selected randomly each session.
After baseline, six repetitions of 30 s relax and lower blocks were completed in a boxcar-design. Both blocks were accompanied with binaural auditory stimulation using the same continuous noise from the functional localizer. During relax, every participant was instructed to relax and clear their mind, resulting in an increase in the feedback signal. They were also instructed to keep their eyes open. Participants were instructed to lower the feedback signal during lower blocks by performing a mindfulness task wherein they should decrease brain activity associated with auditory input. A list of four example mindfulness tasks was provided, giving the participants a few starting points (mindful meditation, thinking about a hobby, doing a mentally engaging task such as math, or thinking about other senses). Through training, participants learned mindfulness tasks that are most successful in regulating A1. Task instructions indicating the current block (relax or lower) were supplied above the thermometer plot.
Participants were then removed from the MRI and escorted out of the MRI room. Participants were then informally interviewed by the experimenter.
The BOLD data acquired from each closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation run was processed using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL) [44,45] on a 72-core Rocks Cluster Distribution (www.rocksclusters.org) high-performance computing system capable of running 120 threads in parallel. First, individual (first-level) analyses were conducted on each of the 4D fMRI data sets. Prior to the individual analyses, t pre-processing was performed using standard techniques. These consisted of applying a high-pass temporal filter (Gaussian-weighted least-squares straight line fitting, cut-off = 60 s) to each voxel, correcting for motion by registering each volume to the center volume of the data set (rigid-body 12-parameter model) [46], creating a brain mask from the first volume and applying to each subsequent volume [47], spatial filtering on each volume using Gaussian convolution (full-width half-maximum of 5.625 mm), and removing low-frequency trends using a local fit of a straight line across time at each voxel with Gaussian weighting within the line to create a smooth response.
Next, individual analyses were conducted on each of the 4D fMRI data sets. A single EV was defined by convolving a boxcar model containing 30 s relax and lower conditions with a HRF (modeled by a gamma function; phase offset = 0 s, standard deviation = 3 s, mean lag = 6 s). The temporal derivative of the original waveform was added to the result. The temporal filter used in pre-processing was applied to the model. The data set was fit to the model using a GLM with prewhitening by applying a weight of −1 to the EV, representative of de-activation during closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation. Z statistic maps were created using standard statistical transforms to convert the β parameter maps. A clustering method allowed us to account for false positives due to multiple comparisons. This method considers adjacent voxels with a z statistic of 2.3 or greater to be a cluster. The significance of each cluster was estimated and compared to a threshold of p < 0.05 using Gaussian Random Field theory. The significance of voxels that either did not pass the significance level threshold or do not belong to a cluster were set to zero. A mean image of the data set was registered to the individual's high-resolution structural image by estimating motion from a boundary-based registration method including a fieldmap-based distortion correction [48], then further registered to the MNI-152 T1-weighted 2 mm template provided in FSL [49,50] using a 12-parameter model. The z statistic maps were converted to standard space using the transform responsible for morphing the mean image of each data set to the template to co-register all volumes. A similar process was performed on the BOLD data acquired during the auditory localizer, but a temporal filter with a cut-off of 40 s and a boxcar model with 20 s conditions were used.
The target ROI coordinates in each fMRI-NFT session were converted to a binary mask. Since the ROI was determined from the first volume of the functional localizer, motion was corrected in the functional localizer data by registering each volume to the first volume using the method described above and a mean image was created. Next, the mean image of each neuromodulation run was registered to the mean image of the associated functional localizer using a rigid-body 12-parameter model. The transform responsible for morphing the mean image of each neuromodulation run was applied to the associated ROI mask. Volitional down-regulation of A1 was assessed in both groups by masking the de-activation map (i.e., decreased BOLD signal during the lower condition compared with the relax condition) from above with the registered ROI mask. A mixed factor ANOVA (between-subjects factor: Group; within-subjects factors: Session and run) was performed on the neuromodulation performance metric using SPSS (IBM SPSS statistics version 24.0, IBM Corp., Amonk, New York).
Group (second level) analyses were performed in FSL using to conduct a voxel-wise 2 × 2 (between subject factor: Group; within-subjects factor: Session) mixed factor ANOVA in FSL. Run 1 from the first fMRI-NFT session and run 2 from the last fMRI-NFT session were included to assess the overall change in A1 de-activation. Prior to running this analysis, each individual de-activation map was masked to remove activated voxels. This enabled us to assess only changes in de-activation, as the results of the ANOVA are bi-directional. The 2 × 2 ANOVA analysis assumed the covariance between measures within-subject follow a compound symmetric structure (equal variance and intra-subject correlations being equal). This assumption is valid as the data was acquired in close proximity and regularly sampled. Two contrasts were created to identify voxels more de-active during the fifth training session than the first session and a larger change in de-activation from the first to fifth training session (5-1) for the EXP group than the CON group. Z statistic maps, created by transforming the resulting β parameter maps using standard statistical transforms, were thresholded using the clustering method outlined above with a z statistic threshold of 1.96. Furthermore, β parameter estimates from each of these contrasts underwent separate F tests to explore the main effect of session and the session by group interaction. This analysis lacked the degrees of freedom necessary to include the main effect of group and, therefore, this contrast was not included. Z statistic images were created from F statistic images using standard statistical transformations. This group analysis was repeated using the auditory localizer from sessions 1 and 5.
Using an independent samples t-test, the mean age for each the EXP group was found to not significantly differ from the CON group (t25 = 1.447, p = 0.160, two-tailed). Equal variances were assumed as Levene's test was not significant (F1.25 = 3.832, p = 0.062). Furthermore, the training time, calculated as the separation between the first and last neurofeedback session, did not significantly differ between the EXP and CON groups (t25 = −0.522, p = 0.606, two-tailed). Equal variances were assumed as Levene's test was not significant (F1.25 = 1.278, p = 0.269).
A mixed factor ANOVA evaluated the size (overall average = 1490 mm3) of the functionally-defined ROI across sessions and groups (Figure 2). The size of the ROIs did not significantly differ between sessions or groups (p = 0.567, p = 0.108, respectively, two-tailed). Furthermore, the interaction of session by group was not significant in the ROI size (p = 0.713, two-tailed). Although the ROIs for the CON group were not used during neurofeedback, these ROIs were utilized for post-processing to compute A1 de-activation. The average size of each ROI across groups and sessions was 1490 mm3 ± 283.15 mm3. Furthermore, the 3D coordinates of the center of mass (COM) or each ROI was computed from the standard-space transformed ROI per session and hemisphere. A total of six 5 × 2 mixed factor ANOVAs evaluated the COM location for each dimension and hemisphere across sessions and groups. The main effect of session was significant for only the z-dimension in the right hemisphere (F4,100 = 5.098, p = 0.001). Bonferroni-corrected post hoc comparisons revealed the z-location at session 2 varied significantly from sessions 1 and 4 (p = 0.003 and p = 0.002, respectively). The main effect of session was not significant for any of the other dimensions/hemispheres (p > 0.05). The main effects of group and the group by session interactions were not significant (p > 0.05) for all three dimensions and both hemispheres, suggesting the location of the selected ROIs were consistent across groups and the small variation in the right z-dimension did not vary differently between groups.
The effects of group, session, and run on A1 de-activation during closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation were evaluated using a mixed factor ANOVA. A1 de-activation is representative of an individual's ability to volitionally down-regulate A1. The results of the tests of between-subjects effects (Table 1) revealed a significant main effect of group (p = 0.029, one-tailed). One-tailed statistics are reported (the a priori hypothesis was that A1 de-activation would be greater in the EXP group). The ANOVA analysis included Mauchly's Test of Sphericity which determined that the variances of the differences between all possible pairs of within-subject conditions were not significant for the main effect of session (p = 0.160, two-tailed) or the interaction of session and run (p = 0.776, two-tailed). This test could not be conducted on the main effect of run because there is only a single difference to compute and, therefore, no comparison to be made. These results validate the assumption of sphericity, which was used to assess the results of the within-subjects tests henceforth. The results of the within-subjects testing (Table 2) identify a significant main effect of session (Figure 3; p = 0.0175, one-tailed). One-tailed statistics are reported (the a priori hypothesis was that A1 de-activation would increase with training). The main effect of run was not significant (p = 0.283, one-tailed). The interaction effects of session by group, run by group, session by run, and session by group and run were not significant (p = 0.225, p = 0.175, p = 0.070, and p = 0.218, respectively).
Source | Type III Sum of Squares | df | Mean Square | F | Sig. (one-tailed) | Partial Eta Squared | Observed Power |
Intercept | 22.381 | 1 | 22.381 | 6.073 | 0.011 | 0.195 | 0.659 |
Group | 14.524 | 1 | 14.524 | 3.941 | 0.029 | 0.136 | 0.480 |
Error | 92.135 | 25 | 3.685 |
Factor | Type III Sum of Squares | df | Mean Square | F | Sig. (one-tailed) | Partial Eta Squared | Observed Power |
Session | 59.395 | 4 | 14.849 | 2.702 | 0.0175 | 0.098 | 0.731 |
Session* Group | 20.447 | 4 | 5.112 | 0.930 | 0.225 | 0.036 | 0.286 |
Run | 0.933 | 1 | 0.933 | 0.338 | 0.283 | 0.013 | 0.087 |
Run* Group | 2.506 | 1 | 2.506 | 0.908 | 0.175 | 0.035 | 0.150 |
Session* Run | 11.377 | 4 | 2.844 | 1.772 | 0.070 | 0.066 | 0.524 |
Session* Run* Group | 6.121 | 4 | 1.530 | 0.953 | 0.218 | 0.037 | 0.292 |
Post hoc, Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons were conducted on the session by group interaction. These results revealed no significant difference between session 1 and 5 for the CON group (p > 0.05; Table 3); however, a significant difference between these sessions was identified in the EXP group (p = 0.0165, one-tailed). There was also a significant difference between sessions 1 and 2 for the EXP group (p = 0.038, one-tailed). Furthermore, the EXP group was found to have significantly greater A1 de-activation than the CON groups on session 2 (p = 0.031, one-tailed) and 5 (p = 0.021, one-tailed) as identified in Table 4.
Group | (I) Session | (J) Session | Mean Difference (I-J) | Std. Error | Sig. (one-tailed) |
CON | 1 | 2 | −0.563 | 0.730 | 1.000 |
3 | −1.024 | 0.802 | 1.000 | ||
4 | −0.949 | 0.813 | 1.000 | ||
5 | −0.597 | 0.978 | 1.000 | ||
2 | 3 | −0.461 | 0.719 | 1.000 | |
4 | −0.386 | 0.828 | 1.000 | ||
5 | −0.034 | 0.829 | 1.000 | ||
3 | 4 | 0.076 | 0.742 | 1.000 | |
5 | 0.427 | 0.739 | 1.000 | ||
4 | 5 | 0.352 | 0.571 | 1.000 | |
EXP | 1 | 2 | −1.496 | 0.516 | 0.038 |
3 | −1.339 | 0.567 | 0.132 | ||
4 | −1.369 | 0.575 | 0.125 | ||
5 | −2.266 | 0.692 | 0.0165 | ||
2 | 3 | 0.157 | 0.509 | 1.000 | |
4 | 0.126 | 0.586 | 1.000 | ||
5 | −0.770 | 0.586 | 1.000 | ||
3 | 4 | −0.030 | 0.525 | 1.000 | |
5 | −0.927 | 0.523 | 0.441 | ||
4 | 5 | −0.897 | 0.404 | 0.178 |
Session | (I) Group | (J) Group | Mean Difference (I-J) | Std. Error | Sig. (one-tailed) |
1 | EXP | CON | 0.888 | 0.585 | 0.071 |
2 | EXP | CON | 1.821 | 0.931 | 0.031 |
3 | EXP | CON | 1.203 | 1.121 | 0.147 |
4 | EXP | CON | 1.309 | 1.007 | 0.103 |
5 | EXP | CON | 2.557 | 1.194 | 0.021 |
A 2 × 2 (group by session) mixed factor ANOVA was performed on the BOLD data from the session 1 run 1 and session 5 run 2 closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation runs using FSL. The F test revealed a significant (z > 1.96) main effect of session on de-activation magnitude during closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation in several regions throughout the brain (Figure 4; Table 5). Increased de-activation was observed across training in auditory regions (superior temporal gyrus, transverse temporal gyrus, and insula) limited to the right hemisphere. In contrast, increased de-activation in attention-related regions (medial frontal gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, and middle frontal gyrus) were observed in the left hemisphere. However, bilateral changes in the anterior cingulate and caudate were observed.
Coordinates | |||
Z statistic | X (mm) | Y (mm) | Z (mm) |
3.82 | 62 | −16 | 42 |
3.55 | −26 | 46 | 38 |
3.4 | −32 | 40 | 40 |
3.36 | 54 | −42 | 24 |
3.33 | 2 | 38 | 12 |
3.3 | 60 | −36 | 24 |
3.22 | 4 | 54 | 40 |
3.15 | 30 | −20 | 12 |
3.15 | 0 | 34 | 6 |
3.14 | 28 | 6 | 10 |
3.14 | −40 | 28 | 22 |
Additional pairwise comparisons revealed a large increase in de-activation magnitude for the EXP group (Figure 5, top row; Table 6). This was apparent in both magnitude and extent. Increases in de-activation magnitude were also observed in the CON group (Figure 5, bottom row; Table 7), however these effects were smaller and more focal than in the EXP group.
Coordinates | |||
Z statistic | X (mm) | Y (mm) | Z (mm) |
4.31 | 10 | −86 | 4 |
3.97 | 62 | −18 | 42 |
3.86 | −66 | −12 | 4 |
3.85 | 34 | 18 | −22 |
3.83 | −14 | 104 | 18 |
3.74 | −12 | −58 | 68 |
An additional 2 × 2 (group by session) mixed factor ANOVA was performed on the BOLD data using the session 1 and 5 auditory localizer using FSL. The F test revealed a significant (z > 1.96) main effect of session on activation magnitude during auditory stimulation in the right inferior frontal gyrus and bilaterally in the superior temporal gyrus (Figure 6). The contrast identifying voxels with significantly greater activation in session 1 compared to session 5 was assessed to clarify the directionality. This contrast identified the regions in the right inferior frontal gyrus and bilateral superior temporal gyrus, implying activation in these regions significantly decreased with training. However, there were no significant findings in the F test for the session by group interaction and, thus, these changes were not found to vary significantly between groups and, thus, suggests the differences in neurofeedback performance cannot be explained by variations in the auditory localizer.
Coordinates | |||
Z statistic | X (mm) | Y (mm) | Z (mm) |
3.45 | −45 | −54 | 28 |
3.4 | −56 | −32 | −16 |
3.33 | 28 | −12 | 8 |
3.23 | −20 | 64 | 24 |
3.23 | −52 | 46 | −10 |
3.17 | 22 | −98 | −8 |
Training self-regulation of brain activity from fMRI-NFT has shown promise in a broad range of applications such as the improvement of human performance [15,16,27] and a variety of medical applications including recovery from stroke [18,20], major depression [32,51], Parkinson's disease [31], and chronic pain [29]. Of the techniques currently being explored, endogenous neuromodulation techniques [6,44,52] have the advantages of no known side effects and may be translated to exercises that could be performed at home without the use of sophisticated equipment and trained professionals [11,32]. Real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging [53,54] has seen a dramatic rise in interest since its advent in 1995, with a large portion of research dedicated to its application for training endogenous neuromodulation. In this technique, termed closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation, the BOLD signal is measured from a specific region of the brain, processed, and presented to the subject in real-time. Through training, subjects develop self-directed mental processing techniques that regulate this signal.
The present study found evidence for successful down-regulation of A1 using fMRI-NFT. The experimental group attempted down-regulation with the aid of real information regarding the current BOLD signals in A1 while the control group was supplied sham feedback yoked from a random participant in the experimental group and matched for training progress. In both groups, the bilateral A1 was identified both anatomically and functionally using an activation map produced during binaural continuous noise stimulation at each of the five training sessions. The results indicate an overall increase in the ability to volitionally decrease A1 activity across training. The most successful participants reported focusing on breathing during “lower” conditions during neurofeedback. A1 de-activation was not found to be significantly different at the first session between the experimental and control groups. However, the ability to volitionally decrease A1 activity was observed to be significantly greater for the experimental group compared to the control group at sessions two and five. Furthermore, self-control over A1 de-activation between the first and last training session was significantly increased in the experimental group. There was also a significant increase between the first and second training session signifying a rapid effect of neurofeedback training on A1 de-activation. These effects were not observed in the control group.
Interestingly, attempting volitional down-regulation of the auditory cortex resulted in a right-lateralized increase of de-activation in the occipital cortex (Figure 3). Asymmetry in the auditory system is well-documented (i.e., “right ear advantage”), thought to be largely due to language processing regions contained in the left hemisphere. Typically, right ear advantage would be observed in the left hemisphere, as the sensory tracts largely decussate so that right ear information is almost entirely processed in the left hemisphere. However, due to the assessment of de-activation in the present study, the right ear advantage might reflect the right-lateralized results. There is a potential for handedness to also play a role similar to the right ear advantage, with similar logic regarding results apparent in de-activation. Additionally, habituation to the noise could be a contributing factor, though unlikely as habituation would have manifested as decreased activation in auditory regions equally apparent in both lower and relax conditions of the neurofeedback training. However, since this result arouse in both groups on average (main effect of session), our study lacks the control group necessary to disregard the potential of these effects as a result from habituation.
Our results add to a growing body of research that demonstrates the success of fMRI-NFT in teaching individuals to self-regulate localized brain activity. A previous controlled study indicates healthy individuals can learn to control the activated cortical volume in the primary and secondary auditory cortex using fMRI-NFT [23]. A second previous study indicated that control over the magnitude of A1 activation is also achievable however not necessarily attributable to fMRI-NFT [21]. The results above add to these previous studies by indicating fMRI-NFT aids control over the magnitude of A1 de-activation. In addition, this result shows that 60 min of distributed fMRI-NFT is adequate to train volitional A1 down-regulation, but significant observable effects are prevalent after only 24 min of training.
Our findings are important in the search for a possible treatment and/or therapy for tinnitus. Tinnitus, the phantom perception of sound, is often a symptom of an underlying condition such as age-related hearing loss, ear injury, or a circulatory system disorder. The phantom noise is highly variable in laterality, pulsatility, percept (pitch, intensity), and duration. Furthermore, a central mechanism has been implied as the percept remains following the complete resection of the auditory nerve [55] and acoustic tumors [56]. Tinnitus has been associated with hyperactivity in the auditory cortex in response to auditory stimulation [57,58] and at rest [59,60,61,62]. Additionally, altered attentional processes has been implicated as the source of the percept [58,63]. Only one previous study has investigated fMRI-NFT as a possible treatment for tinnitus [21]. In their study, four 4 min closed-loop endogenous neuromodulation runs to train up-regulation of A1 activation were completed in a single training session. The behavioral assessments were conducted before and after the single fMRI-NFT session. Their study indicates the promise of fMRI-NFT in treating tinnitus, but only included six participants and did not offer a control group. Furthermore, the researchers did not perform any statistical analysis on the behavioral data. Our study is unique for two reasons. First, we employed fMRI-NFT to reduce A1 activity which, as the literature suggests, is hyperactive in tinnitus populations. Second, A1 activity was reduced by directing attention away from the auditory cortex. This is important since it has been suggested that over-attention is drawn toward auditory processing in tinnitus populations.
The results presented in this work align with previous findings which indicate fMRI-NFT can be used to teach participants to voluntarily control the auditory cortex. However, the results of the presented work add to the previous findings by indicating volitional down-regulation of the auditory cortex is achievable in the presence of continuous noise using fMRI neurofeedback. This has not been previously reported. Tinnitus can cause severe impairments and can even limit the ability to perform daily functions. The financial burden associated with tinnitus is extensive. The number of U.S. veterans receiving service-connected disability for tinnitus exceeded all other disorders including post-traumatic stress disorder, hearing loss, and major depression [Annual Benefits Report Fiscal Year 2014, U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs]. The tinnitus percept is attributed to a central mechanism. Also, tinnitus has been associated with hyperactivity in the auditory cortex and abnormal attentional processes (theorized to cause the tinnitus percept). The results presented suggest attempting down-regulation of the auditory cortex may be a possible treatment for tinnitus by decreasing hyperactivity of the primary auditory cortex and directing attention away from auditory processing. Future work is necessary to study these procedures in a cohort of tinnitus patients but should also assess changes in activation associated with volitional down-regulation of the auditory cortex.
This material is based on research sponsored by the U.S. Air Force under agreement number FA8650-16-2-6702. The views expressed are those of the authors and do not reflect the official views or policy of the Department of Defense and its Components. The U.S. Government is authorized to reproduce and distribute reprints for Governmental purposes notwithstanding any copyright notation thereon. The voluntary, fully informed consent of the subjects used in this research was obtained as required by 32 CFR 219 and DODI 3216.02_AFI 40-402.
The authors declare that this research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
[1] | Hirsch J, Ruge MI, Kim KH, et al. (2000) An integrated functional magnetic resonance imaging procedure for preoperative mapping of cortical areas associated with tactile, motor, language, and visual functions. Neurosurgery 47: 711–722. |
[2] |
Yoo SS, Fairneny T, Chen NK, et al. (2004) Brain computer interface using fMRI: Spatial navigation by thoughts. Neuroreport 15: 1591–1595. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000133296.39160.fe
![]() |
[3] |
Sorger B, Reithler J, Dahmen B, et al. (2012) A real-time fMRI-based spelling device immediately enabling robust motor-independent communication. Curr Biol 22: 1333–1338. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.022
![]() |
[4] |
Yoo JJ, Hinds O, Ofen N, et al. (2012) When the brain is prepared to learn: Enhancing human learning using real-time fMRI. Neuroimage 59: 846–852. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.063
![]() |
[5] |
Weiskopf N, Veit R, Erb M, et al. (2003) Physiological self-regulation of regional brain activity using real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): Methodology and exemplary data. Neuroimage 19: 577–586. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00145-9
![]() |
[6] |
Mak JN, Wolpaw JR (2009) Clinical applications of brain-computer interfaces: Current state and future prospects. IEEE Rev Biomed Eng 2: 187–199. doi: 10.1109/RBME.2009.2035356
![]() |
[7] |
Logothetis NK, Pauls J, Augath M, et al. (2001) Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature 412: 150–157. doi: 10.1038/35084005
![]() |
[8] |
Longden TA, Dabertrand F, Koide M, et al. (2017) Capillary K+-sensing initiates retrograde hyperpolarization to increase local cerebral blood flow. Nat Neurosci 20: 717–726. doi: 10.1038/nn.4533
![]() |
[9] |
Hamilton JP, Glover GH, Hsu JJ, et al. (2011) Modulation of subgenual anterior cingulate cortex activity with real-time neurofeedback. Hum Brain Mapp 32: 22–31. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20997
![]() |
[10] |
Zotev V, Krueger F, Phillips R, et al. (2011) Self-regulation of amygdala activation using real-time fMRI neurofeedback. PLoS One 6: e24522. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024522
![]() |
[11] |
Caria A, Veit R, Sitaram R, et al. (2007) Regulation of anterior insular cortex activity using real-time fMRI. Neuroimage 35: 1238–1246. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.018
![]() |
[12] |
Veit R, Singh V, Sitaram R, et al. (2012) Using real-time fMRI to learn voluntary regulation of the anterior insula in the presence of threat-related stimuli. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 7: 623–634. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr061
![]() |
[13] |
Lee JH, Kim J, Yoo SS (2012) Real-time fMRI-based neurofeedback reinforces causality of attention networks. Neurosci Res 72: 347–354. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2012.01.002
![]() |
[14] |
Mccaig RG, Dixon M, Keramatian K, et al. (2011) Improved modulation of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex using real-time fMRI training and meta-cognitive awareness. Neuroimage 55: 1298–1305. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.016
![]() |
[15] |
Zhang G, Yao L, Zhang H, et al. (2013) Improved working memory performance through self-regulation of dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex activation using real-time fMRI. PLoS One 8: e73735. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073735
![]() |
[16] |
Sherwood MS, Kane JH, Weisend MP, et al. (2016) Enhanced control of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex neurophysiology with real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging (rt-fMRI) neurofeedback training and working memory practice. Neuroimage 124: 214–223. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.074
![]() |
[17] | Sherwood MS, Weisend MP, Kane JH, et al. (2016) Combining real-time fMRI neurofeedback training of the DLPFC with N-Back practice results in neuroplastic effects confined to the neurofeedback target region. Front Behav Neurosci 10: 1–9. |
[18] |
Sitaram R, Veit R, Stevens B, et al. (2012) Acquired control of ventral premotor cortex activity by feedback training: An exploratory real-time fMRI and TMS study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 26: 256–265. doi: 10.1177/1545968311418345
![]() |
[19] |
Berman BD, Horovitz SG, Venkataraman G, et al. (2012) Self-modulation of primary motor cortex activity with motor and motor imagery tasks using real-time fMRI-based neurofeedback. Neuroimage 59: 917–925. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.035
![]() |
[20] |
Chiew M, Laconte SM, Graham SJ (2012) Investigation of fMRI neurofeedback of differential primary motor cortex activity using kinesthetic motor imagery. Neuroimage 61: 21–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.053
![]() |
[21] |
Haller S, Birbaumer N, Veit R (2010) Real-time fMRI feedback training may improve chronic tinnitus. Eur Radiol 20: 696–703. doi: 10.1007/s00330-009-1595-z
![]() |
[22] |
Haller S, Kopel R, Jhooti P, et al. (2013) Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain functional networks through neurofeedback. Neuroimage 81: 243–252. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.019
![]() |
[23] |
Yoo SS, O'Leary HM, Fairneny T, et al. (2006) Increasing cortical activity in auditory areas through neurofeedback functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroreport 17: 1273–1278. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000227996.53540.22
![]() |
[24] |
Johnston S, Linden DE, Healy D, et al. (2011) Upregulation of emotion areas through neurofeedback with a focus on positive mood. Cogn it Affective Behav Neurosci 11: 44–51. doi: 10.3758/s13415-010-0010-1
![]() |
[25] |
Johnston SJ, Boehm SG, Healy D, et al. (2010) Neurofeedback: A promising tool for the self-regulation of emotion networks. Neuroimage 49: 1066–1072. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.056
![]() |
[26] | Rota G, Sitaram R, Veit R, et al. (2010) Self-regulation of regional cortical activity using real-time fMRI: The right inferior frontal gyrus and linguistic processing. Hum Brain Mapp 30: 1605–1614. |
[27] |
Scharnowski F, Hutton C, Josephs O, et al. (2012) Improving visual perception through neurofeedback. J Neurosci 32: 17830–17841. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6334-11.2012
![]() |
[28] |
Shibata K, Kawato M (2011) Perceptual learning incepted by decoded fmri neurofeedback without stimulus presentation. Science 334: 1413–1415. doi: 10.1126/science.1212003
![]() |
[29] |
Decharms RC, Maeda F, Glover GH, et al. (2005) Control over brain activation and pain learned by using real-time functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102: 18626–18631. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505210102
![]() |
[30] |
Ruiz S, Lee S, Soekadar SR, et al. (2013) Acquired self-control of insula cortex modulates emotion recognition and brain network connectivity in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp 34: 200–212. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21427
![]() |
[31] |
Subramanian L, Hindle JV, Johnston S, et al. (2011) Real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback for treatment of Parkinson's disease. J Neurosci 31: 16309–16317. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3498-11.2011
![]() |
[32] |
Linden DEJ, Habes I, Johnston SJ, et al. (2012) Real-time self-regulation of emotion networks in patients with depression. PLoS One 7: e38115. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038115
![]() |
[33] |
Decharms RC, Christoff K, Glover GH, et al. (2004) Learned regulation of spatially localized brain activation using real-time fMRI. Neuroimage 21: 436–443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.041
![]() |
[34] |
Yoo SS, Lee JH, O'Leary H, et al. (2008) Neurofeedback fMRI-mediated learning and consolidation of regional brain activation during motor imagery. Int J Imaging Syst Technol 18: 69–78. doi: 10.1002/ima.20139
![]() |
[35] |
Birbaumer N, Cohen LG (2007) Brain-computer interfaces: Communication and restoration of movement in paralysis. J Physiol 579: 621–636. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.125633
![]() |
[36] |
Daly JJ, Wolpaw JR (2008) Brain-computer interfaces in neurological rehabilitation. Lancet Neurol 7: 1032–1043. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70223-0
![]() |
[37] |
Ros T, Munneke MAM, Ruge D, et al. (2010) Endogenous control of waking brain rhythms induces neuroplasticity in humans. Eur J Neurosci 31: 770–778. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07100.x
![]() |
[38] |
Orlov ND, Giampietro V, O'Daly O, et al. (2018) Real-time fMRI neurofeedback to down-regulate superior temporal gyrus activity in patients with schizophrenia and auditory hallucinations: A proof-of-concept study. Transl Psychiatry 8: 46. doi: 10.1038/s41398-017-0067-5
![]() |
[39] | Paret C, Kluetsch R, Ruf M, et al. (2014) Down-regulation of amygdala activation with real-time fMRI neurofeedback in a healthy female sample. Front Behav Neurosci 8: 299. |
[40] | Saliba J, Al-Reefi M, Carriere JS, et al. (2016) Accuracy of mobile-based audiometry in the evaluation of hearing loss in quiet and noisy environments. Otolaryngol Neck Surg 156: 706–711. |
[41] |
Thompson GP, Sladen DP, Borst BJ, et al. (2015) Accuracy of a tablet audiometer for measuring behavioral hearing thresholds in a clinical population. Otolaryngol Neck Surg 153: 838–842. doi: 10.1177/0194599815593737
![]() |
[42] |
Worsley KJ, Friston KJ (1995) Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited-again. Neuroimage 2: 173–181. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1023
![]() |
[43] | Ashby F Gregory (2011) Statistical analysis of fMRI data. MIT press. |
[44] | Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, et al. (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Math Brain Imaging 23: S208–S219. |
[45] | Woolrich MW, Jbabdi S, Patenaude B, et al. (2009) Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Math Brain Imaging 45: S173–S186. |
[46] |
Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, et al. (2002) Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17: 825–841. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132
![]() |
[47] |
Smith SM (2002) Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp 17: 143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062
![]() |
[48] |
Greve DN, Fischl B (2009) Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. Neuroimage 48: 63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060
![]() |
[49] | Collins DL, Holmes CJ, Peters TM, et al. (2004) Automatic 3-D model-based neuroanatomical segmentation. Hum Brain Mapp 3: 190–208. |
[50] | Mazziotta J, Toga A, Evans A, et al. (2002) A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain: International Consortium for Brain Mapping (ICBM). Brain Mapp Methods 356: 1293–1322. |
[51] |
Young KD, Zotev V, Phillips R, et al. (2014) Real-time fMRI neurofeedback training of amygdala activity in patients with major depressive disorder. PLoS One 9: e88785. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088785
![]() |
[52] |
Sulzer J, Haller S, Scharnowski F, et al. (2013) Real-time fMRI neurofeedback: Progress and challenges. Neuroimage 76: 386–399. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.033
![]() |
[53] |
Cox RW, Jesmanowicz A, Hyde JS (1995) Real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging. Magn Reson Med 33: 230–236. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910330213
![]() |
[54] | Weiskopf N, Sitaram R, Josephs O, et al. (2007) Real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging: Methods and applications. Proc Int Sch Magn Reson Brain Funct 25: 989–1003. |
[55] |
Folmer RL, Griest S, Martin W (2001) Chronic tinnitus as phantom auditory pain. Otolaryngol-Head Neck Surg 124: 394–400. doi: 10.1067/mhn.2001.114673
![]() |
[56] | Berliner KI, Shelton C, Hitselberger WE, et al. (1992) Acoustic tumors: Effect of surgical removal on tinnitus. Otol Neurotol 13: 13. |
[57] |
Gu JW, Halpin CF, Nam EC, et al. (2010) Tinnitus, diminished sound-level tolerance, and elevated auditory activity in humans with clinically normal hearing sensitivity. J Neurophysiol 104: 3361–3370. doi: 10.1152/jn.00226.2010
![]() |
[58] |
Seydellgreenwald A, Leaver AM, Turesky TK, et al. (2012) Functional MRI evidence for a role of ventral prefrontal cortex in tinnitus. Brain Ress 1485: 22–39. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.08.052
![]() |
[59] | Wang H, Tian J, Yin D, et al. (2001) Regional glucose metabolic increases in left auditory cortex in tinnitus patients: A preliminary study with positron emission tomography. Chin Med J 114: 848–851. |
[60] |
Langguth B, Eichhammer P, Kreutzer A, et al. (2006) The impact of auditory cortex activity on characterizing and treating patients with chronic tinnitus-first results from a PET study. Acta Otolaryngol 126: 84–88. doi: 10.1080/03655230600895317
![]() |
[61] |
Schecklmann M, Landgrebe M, Poeppl TB, et al. (2013) Neural correlates of tinnitus duration and Distress: A positron emission tomography study. Hum Brain Mapp 34: 233–240. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21426
![]() |
[62] |
Geven LI, de Kleine E, Willemsen ATM, et al. (2014) Asymmetry in primary auditory cortex activity in tinnitus patients and controls. Neuroscience 256: 117–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.10.015
![]() |
[63] |
Kim SG, Ogawa S (2012) Biophysical and physiological origins of blood oxygenation level-dependent fMRI signals. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 32: 1188–1206. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2012.23
![]() |
1. | Samantha J. Fede, Sarah F. Dean, Thushini Manuweera, Reza Momenan, A Guide to Literature Informed Decisions in the Design of Real Time fMRI Neurofeedback Studies: A Systematic Review, 2020, 14, 1662-5161, 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00060 | |
2. | David Linden, 2021, 9780128224212, 249, 10.1016/B978-0-12-822421-2.00003-X | |
3. | Patrick Neff, Martin Meyer, 2024, Chapter 51, 978-3-031-35646-9, 653, 10.1007/978-3-031-35647-6_51 | |
4. | Joseph CC. Chen, David A. Ziegler, Closed-Loop Systems and Real-Time Neurofeedback in Mindfulness Meditation Research, 2024, 24519022, 10.1016/j.bpsc.2024.10.012 |
Source | Type III Sum of Squares | df | Mean Square | F | Sig. (one-tailed) | Partial Eta Squared | Observed Power |
Intercept | 22.381 | 1 | 22.381 | 6.073 | 0.011 | 0.195 | 0.659 |
Group | 14.524 | 1 | 14.524 | 3.941 | 0.029 | 0.136 | 0.480 |
Error | 92.135 | 25 | 3.685 |
Factor | Type III Sum of Squares | df | Mean Square | F | Sig. (one-tailed) | Partial Eta Squared | Observed Power |
Session | 59.395 | 4 | 14.849 | 2.702 | 0.0175 | 0.098 | 0.731 |
Session* Group | 20.447 | 4 | 5.112 | 0.930 | 0.225 | 0.036 | 0.286 |
Run | 0.933 | 1 | 0.933 | 0.338 | 0.283 | 0.013 | 0.087 |
Run* Group | 2.506 | 1 | 2.506 | 0.908 | 0.175 | 0.035 | 0.150 |
Session* Run | 11.377 | 4 | 2.844 | 1.772 | 0.070 | 0.066 | 0.524 |
Session* Run* Group | 6.121 | 4 | 1.530 | 0.953 | 0.218 | 0.037 | 0.292 |
Group | (I) Session | (J) Session | Mean Difference (I-J) | Std. Error | Sig. (one-tailed) |
CON | 1 | 2 | −0.563 | 0.730 | 1.000 |
3 | −1.024 | 0.802 | 1.000 | ||
4 | −0.949 | 0.813 | 1.000 | ||
5 | −0.597 | 0.978 | 1.000 | ||
2 | 3 | −0.461 | 0.719 | 1.000 | |
4 | −0.386 | 0.828 | 1.000 | ||
5 | −0.034 | 0.829 | 1.000 | ||
3 | 4 | 0.076 | 0.742 | 1.000 | |
5 | 0.427 | 0.739 | 1.000 | ||
4 | 5 | 0.352 | 0.571 | 1.000 | |
EXP | 1 | 2 | −1.496 | 0.516 | 0.038 |
3 | −1.339 | 0.567 | 0.132 | ||
4 | −1.369 | 0.575 | 0.125 | ||
5 | −2.266 | 0.692 | 0.0165 | ||
2 | 3 | 0.157 | 0.509 | 1.000 | |
4 | 0.126 | 0.586 | 1.000 | ||
5 | −0.770 | 0.586 | 1.000 | ||
3 | 4 | −0.030 | 0.525 | 1.000 | |
5 | −0.927 | 0.523 | 0.441 | ||
4 | 5 | −0.897 | 0.404 | 0.178 |
Session | (I) Group | (J) Group | Mean Difference (I-J) | Std. Error | Sig. (one-tailed) |
1 | EXP | CON | 0.888 | 0.585 | 0.071 |
2 | EXP | CON | 1.821 | 0.931 | 0.031 |
3 | EXP | CON | 1.203 | 1.121 | 0.147 |
4 | EXP | CON | 1.309 | 1.007 | 0.103 |
5 | EXP | CON | 2.557 | 1.194 | 0.021 |
Coordinates | |||
Z statistic | X (mm) | Y (mm) | Z (mm) |
3.82 | 62 | −16 | 42 |
3.55 | −26 | 46 | 38 |
3.4 | −32 | 40 | 40 |
3.36 | 54 | −42 | 24 |
3.33 | 2 | 38 | 12 |
3.3 | 60 | −36 | 24 |
3.22 | 4 | 54 | 40 |
3.15 | 30 | −20 | 12 |
3.15 | 0 | 34 | 6 |
3.14 | 28 | 6 | 10 |
3.14 | −40 | 28 | 22 |
Coordinates | |||
Z statistic | X (mm) | Y (mm) | Z (mm) |
4.31 | 10 | −86 | 4 |
3.97 | 62 | −18 | 42 |
3.86 | −66 | −12 | 4 |
3.85 | 34 | 18 | −22 |
3.83 | −14 | 104 | 18 |
3.74 | −12 | −58 | 68 |
Coordinates | |||
Z statistic | X (mm) | Y (mm) | Z (mm) |
3.45 | −45 | −54 | 28 |
3.4 | −56 | −32 | −16 |
3.33 | 28 | −12 | 8 |
3.23 | −20 | 64 | 24 |
3.23 | −52 | 46 | −10 |
3.17 | 22 | −98 | −8 |
Source | Type III Sum of Squares | df | Mean Square | F | Sig. (one-tailed) | Partial Eta Squared | Observed Power |
Intercept | 22.381 | 1 | 22.381 | 6.073 | 0.011 | 0.195 | 0.659 |
Group | 14.524 | 1 | 14.524 | 3.941 | 0.029 | 0.136 | 0.480 |
Error | 92.135 | 25 | 3.685 |
Factor | Type III Sum of Squares | df | Mean Square | F | Sig. (one-tailed) | Partial Eta Squared | Observed Power |
Session | 59.395 | 4 | 14.849 | 2.702 | 0.0175 | 0.098 | 0.731 |
Session* Group | 20.447 | 4 | 5.112 | 0.930 | 0.225 | 0.036 | 0.286 |
Run | 0.933 | 1 | 0.933 | 0.338 | 0.283 | 0.013 | 0.087 |
Run* Group | 2.506 | 1 | 2.506 | 0.908 | 0.175 | 0.035 | 0.150 |
Session* Run | 11.377 | 4 | 2.844 | 1.772 | 0.070 | 0.066 | 0.524 |
Session* Run* Group | 6.121 | 4 | 1.530 | 0.953 | 0.218 | 0.037 | 0.292 |
Group | (I) Session | (J) Session | Mean Difference (I-J) | Std. Error | Sig. (one-tailed) |
CON | 1 | 2 | −0.563 | 0.730 | 1.000 |
3 | −1.024 | 0.802 | 1.000 | ||
4 | −0.949 | 0.813 | 1.000 | ||
5 | −0.597 | 0.978 | 1.000 | ||
2 | 3 | −0.461 | 0.719 | 1.000 | |
4 | −0.386 | 0.828 | 1.000 | ||
5 | −0.034 | 0.829 | 1.000 | ||
3 | 4 | 0.076 | 0.742 | 1.000 | |
5 | 0.427 | 0.739 | 1.000 | ||
4 | 5 | 0.352 | 0.571 | 1.000 | |
EXP | 1 | 2 | −1.496 | 0.516 | 0.038 |
3 | −1.339 | 0.567 | 0.132 | ||
4 | −1.369 | 0.575 | 0.125 | ||
5 | −2.266 | 0.692 | 0.0165 | ||
2 | 3 | 0.157 | 0.509 | 1.000 | |
4 | 0.126 | 0.586 | 1.000 | ||
5 | −0.770 | 0.586 | 1.000 | ||
3 | 4 | −0.030 | 0.525 | 1.000 | |
5 | −0.927 | 0.523 | 0.441 | ||
4 | 5 | −0.897 | 0.404 | 0.178 |
Session | (I) Group | (J) Group | Mean Difference (I-J) | Std. Error | Sig. (one-tailed) |
1 | EXP | CON | 0.888 | 0.585 | 0.071 |
2 | EXP | CON | 1.821 | 0.931 | 0.031 |
3 | EXP | CON | 1.203 | 1.121 | 0.147 |
4 | EXP | CON | 1.309 | 1.007 | 0.103 |
5 | EXP | CON | 2.557 | 1.194 | 0.021 |
Coordinates | |||
Z statistic | X (mm) | Y (mm) | Z (mm) |
3.82 | 62 | −16 | 42 |
3.55 | −26 | 46 | 38 |
3.4 | −32 | 40 | 40 |
3.36 | 54 | −42 | 24 |
3.33 | 2 | 38 | 12 |
3.3 | 60 | −36 | 24 |
3.22 | 4 | 54 | 40 |
3.15 | 30 | −20 | 12 |
3.15 | 0 | 34 | 6 |
3.14 | 28 | 6 | 10 |
3.14 | −40 | 28 | 22 |
Coordinates | |||
Z statistic | X (mm) | Y (mm) | Z (mm) |
4.31 | 10 | −86 | 4 |
3.97 | 62 | −18 | 42 |
3.86 | −66 | −12 | 4 |
3.85 | 34 | 18 | −22 |
3.83 | −14 | 104 | 18 |
3.74 | −12 | −58 | 68 |
Coordinates | |||
Z statistic | X (mm) | Y (mm) | Z (mm) |
3.45 | −45 | −54 | 28 |
3.4 | −56 | −32 | −16 |
3.33 | 28 | −12 | 8 |
3.23 | −20 | 64 | 24 |
3.23 | −52 | 46 | −10 |
3.17 | 22 | −98 | −8 |