
Citation: Robert Friedman. Themes of advanced information processing in the primate brain[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2020, 7(4): 373-388. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2020023
[1] | Holly Gaff, Elsa Schaefer . Optimal control applied to vaccination and treatment strategies for various epidemiological models. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2009, 6(3): 469-492. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2009.6.469 |
[2] | Salihu S. Musa, Shi Zhao, Winnie Mkandawire, Andrés Colubri, Daihai He . An epidemiological modeling investigation of the long-term changing dynamics of the plague epidemics in Hong Kong. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2024, 21(10): 7435-7453. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2024327 |
[3] | Eunha Shim . Optimal strategies of social distancing and vaccination against seasonal influenza. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2013, 10(5&6): 1615-1634. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2013.10.1615 |
[4] | Antonios Armaou, Bryce Katch, Lucia Russo, Constantinos Siettos . Designing social distancing policies for the COVID-19 pandemic: A probabilistic model predictive control approach. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2022, 19(9): 8804-8832. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2022409 |
[5] | Pannathon Kreabkhontho, Watchara Teparos, Thitiya Theparod . Potential for eliminating COVID-19 in Thailand through third-dose vaccination: A modeling approach. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2024, 21(8): 6807-6828. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2024298 |
[6] | Anjana Pokharel, Khagendra Adhikari, Ramesh Gautam, Kedar Nath Uprety, Naveen K. Vaidya . Modeling transmission dynamics of measles in Nepal and its control with monitored vaccination program. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2022, 19(8): 8554-8579. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2022397 |
[7] | Fernando Saldaña, Hugo Flores-Arguedas, José Ariel Camacho-Gutiérrez, Ignacio Barradas . Modeling the transmission dynamics and the impact of the control interventions for the COVID-19 epidemic outbreak. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2020, 17(4): 4165-4183. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2020231 |
[8] | Glenn Webb, Martin J. Blaser, Huaiping Zhu, Sten Ardal, Jianhong Wu . Critical role of nosocomial transmission in the Toronto SARS outbreak. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2004, 1(1): 1-13. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2004.1.1 |
[9] | Chenwei Song, Rui Xu, Ning Bai, Xiaohong Tian, Jiazhe Lin . Global dynamics and optimal control of a cholera transmission model with vaccination strategy and multiple pathways. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2020, 17(4): 4210-4224. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2020233 |
[10] | Hai-Feng Huo, Tian Fu, Hong Xiang . Dynamics and optimal control of a Zika model with sexual and vertical transmissions. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2023, 20(5): 8279-8304. doi: 10.3934/mbe.2023361 |
Climate change has always been one of the most challenging environmental issues of recent times. Climate change concerns, like global warming, have challenged human communities for sustainable development due to the large amounts of greenhouse gas emissions (GHG). CO2 emissions are the eminent cause of climate change (Lashof & Ahuja, 1990), contributing approximately 80% of the GHGs responsible for global warming. An international agreement addressed these and other growing concerns about global warming—the Kyoto Protocol in 2005 (International Energy Agency, 2012). To keep the climate intact various tactics were used to mitigate climate change. Among all, the European Union Emission Trading Scheme (EU-ETS), which was launched in 2005, represents the biggest carbon trading system in the world. It provides EU members with incentives to mitigate emissions and has become an essential tool for managing CO2 reduction (Bing et al., 2015). The EU-ETS operates through a cap-and-trade program on the total volume of carbon emission allowances allocated per year; companies not corresponding to their CO2 allowances face severe sanctions. A firm may emit less carbon than is allowed resulting in a surplus. This surplus can be sold to companies whose emission is greater than the allowance, making EU allowances a tradable commodity and carbon market as a mimic of the financial market (Benz et al., 2006; Uddin & Holtedahl, 2013).
In addition to the carbon market, Governments are promoting the clean energy sector all around the globe (Kazemilari et al., 2017) to minimize the adverse environmental effects caused by GHG emissions. Previous studies by Balcılar et al. (2016) and Reboredo et al. (2019) suggest that the main reason for the growth in renewable energy is increasing CO2 emissions. Thus, one can say that carbon emission prices may affect the investments of renewable energy stocks. According to Creti et al. (2012), there is a linkage between energy commodities and carbon assets. Several authors have studied this linkage (Convery & Redmond, 2007; Bunn & Fezzi, 2007; Gronwald et al., 2011; Keppler & Mansanet-Bataller, 2010; Kumar et al., 2012; Marimoutou & Soury, 2015; Reboredo, 2014; Reboredo, 2015; Sadorsky, 2012; Tian et al., 2016; Wen et al., 2017; Zhang & Du, 2017; Zhang and Sun, 2016) and found significant effects.
Earlier studies have focused on the relationship between CO2 and fossil fuels, yet the linkage between CO2 allowances and the renewable energy market remains understudied. The effects of the EUA market on renewable energy stocks are initially specified by Kumar et al. (2012). Further, Koch et al. (2014) and Dutta (2018) focused on the return and volatility linkages between carbon emissions and renewable energy stock prices. The existing studies are limited to examine the return and volatility patterns among EUA and clean energy markets; however, it is crucial for investors to seek investment opportunities in alternative asset classes (carbon to clean energy market or vice versa) to reduce their risk. Hence, the main objective of this study is to measure the impact on risk and returns of clean energy stocks if carbon asset is included in the portfolio and examine diversification benefits. This paper contributes to the standing literature in the following ways.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that employs the Generalized Orthogonal-GARCH model to examine the time-varying relationship between the markets under consideration. This model is computationally simpler and has no dimensionality constraints. In addition to GO-GARCH, the symmetric and asymmetric DCCs are applied with a multivariate student-t distribution to check non-normality in the distribution. Then we estimate GO GARCH with a multivariate affine negative inverse Gaussian (MANIG) distribution.
Secondly, it is pioneer research to investigate whether the carbon assets add to the value of clean energy stock portfolios. As earlier studies have indicated, both the clean energy sector and carbon market have seen significant growth and are expected to grow even more in the future, thus, the need for more attention. In addition, it will help investors decide whether to add carbon assets to their portfolios to increase return or not and to what extent these assets can be added for portfolio optimization (Arouri et al., 2015). Thus, these findings will assist investors during portfolio optimization and weight allocation.
Third, this research comparatively measures the diversification benefits of the alternate market (clean energy markets) for the carbon market.
Fourth, this paper is a source of information for policymakers to design energy policies: promoting portfolios comprised of carbon assets and clean energy stocks to reduce GHG emissions further.
Finally, the three phases of carbon allowance are represented in the study, thus making the empirical analysis more comprehensive and robust.
Since the emergence of the EU-ETS in 2005, the body of knowledge on clean energy and the carbon market has shown rapid growth. Considering EUAs as a production factor, Kara et al. (2008) found that 1 euro/ton CO2 change might result in 0.74 euro/MWH rise in power prices. Based on the argument that carbon prices impact the energy sector because of their significant exposure to overall EU CO2 emissions. Therefore, replacing fuel oil, coal, and natural gas influences the price of carbon allowances due to the disparity in carbon emissions through these fossil fuels. Exploring the topic holds immense importance for diversification in portfolio management (Luo & Wu, 2016). Carbon assets can control risk in the portfolio management of energy stocks (Reboredo, 2015). Dutta et al. (2018), Reboredo (2013), Reboredo (2014), and Luo & Wu (2016) suggested that carbon assets have likely diversification benefits due to their relative independence from financial markets. Wen et al. (2017) confirmed the results by creating portfolios that included EUAs.
In contrast, Leitao et al. (2021) argue that green assets such as green bonds significantly influence CO2 prices. The portfolio risk of the portfolios having CO2 futures was lower than that of energy stocks without CO2 futures. Moreover, Andersson et al. (2016) concluded that investors can hedge their risk through carbon allowances as the financial returns are not sacrificed, and the risk is significantly reduced while the tracking error is minimized. These results can be informative for passive investors. Assessing the values of energy commodity futures with the carbons assets, Wen et al. (2017) found that dynamic portfolio diversification is better than the hedged portfolios in reducing carbon risk. Adding to this, Zhang et al. (2017) checked the diversification benefits of including carbon assets in financial portfolios. They first studied the correlation between the carbon market and other financial markets and found a highly significant relationship in the short term, which diminishes in the long run. Consequently, their results revealed that carbon assets help lessen the volatility of an optimal portfolio, but the overall return seems to fall.
Several studies have used copula models to model market dependency. For example, Reboredo et al. (2019) and Reboredo (2013) test the dependency between EUA and crude oil prices. Bai et al. (2019) used a robust portfolio approach to examine the portfolio performance of renewable energy. However, due to its convenience and effectiveness, multivariate GARCH remains the most popular model for analyzing dependency between markets. Various studies have used GARCH models to capture the linkage between carbon variables and other financial markets. Koch et al. (2014) checked the dependency between the allowance market and the energy market with the same approach. Zhang et al. (2017) and Zhang and Sun (2016) analyzed volatility spillovers between carbon and other financial markets through DCC GARCH models. Luo and Wu (2016) applied the GARCH model to investigate the correlation among EUAs, crude oil, and the stock market; however, Mean-Variance and CoVaR methods were used to examine portfolio performance. Moreover, Dutta (2019), Lin and Chen (2019); Lin and Jia (2020) are the recent studies to use multivariate GARCH models.
In the present study, the GO-GARCH model is employed for testing the time-varying dependency between clean energy stock and carbon assets. Furthermore, risk reduction effectiveness and value-at-risk reduction strategies are used for evaluating portfolio performance. Multivariate GARCH models have been frequently used to study the volatility and co-volatility between several markets (Papantonis, 2016). Commonly, there are numerous risk factors in stock indices that need to be modeled through covariance matrix dynamics, making the process of estimating portfolio risk cumbersome. In comparison to other models which possess the above difficulty, GO-GARCH is computationally uncomplicated and yet efficient. By taking few principal components, GARCH can be extended to generalized O-GARCH, which applies calculations to a few main factors, capturing the orthogonal deviations in the root data (Ding, 1994; Lam et al., 2009). Thus, it possesses the simplicity of the univariate model and the efficiency of the multivariate model. Moreover, this approach is superior to multivariate models as it results in no noise in the data, has no dimensionality constraints, and can be applied to even unreliable and scant data (Alexander, 2000). According to Engle (2002), GO-GARCH is preferred to all other alternatives of estimating dynamic covariance matrices.
Introduction in section 1 of the paper covers the background of the study, objectives, significance, and related literature. The remainder of the paper is organized as follows. Section 2 deals with the data and the overview of the methodology. The next section describes the results, followed by the discussion part in section 4. Finally, section 5 consists of the conclusion, implications, and limitations of the study.
We consider the daily prices of carbon assets (EUA) and the clean energy index (ERIX). The EUA in EU-ETS represents the leading carbon trading allowance market in the world, and the ERIX index is the most representative renewable energy market index of Europe. It consists of the top ten largest and most liquid companies in the wind, solar, hydro, and biomass energy sectors.
The data is obtained from Datastream, a glog bal database of Thomson Reuters. The sample period starts from the trading date of the ERIX index from October 13, 2005, to October 13, 2020, covering almost all three phases of EUA (Phase-III ended in December 2020).
Figure 1 is showing the price trends of the EUA and ERIX index, where allowance prices depict greater volatility than the clean energy stocks. In 2008, a sharp decline in the prices can be seen; the possible reason being the global financial crises that severely affected the Eurozone, driving energy demand downwards; thus, reducing the demand for carbon allowances. This decline can again be witnessed in mid-2011 and 2020 due to the economic slowdown in Eurozone and Covid-19 pandemic causing global economic downturns.
Table 1 presents the summary statistics of the return series of both indices. The volatility and range between maximum and minimum values are higher for EUA than ERIX, whereas the mean of both return series is positive and exhibits excess kurtosis.
EUA | ERIX | |
Min | −0.4321 | −0.1297 |
Max | 0.2382 | 0.0795 |
Mean | 0.0067 | 0.0033 |
Std. Dev. | 0.0316 | 0.0164 |
Skewness | −0.8162 | −0.3595 |
Kurtosis | 17.7436 | 6.2519 |
Table 2 presents the Pearson Correlation results of the variable ERIX and EUA. Both the markets are negatively correlated to each other with 10% significance. The correlation value is −0.40 which is negative, implying that adding carbon assets to the clean energy stock portfolio might yield higher value as the markets move in the opposite direction.
ERIX | EUA | |
ERIX | 1 | |
EUA | −0.40* | 1 |
Notes: *Represents significance at 10% level. |
Table 3 shows the unit root diagnostic tests of both markets. The results confirm the stationarity with significant PP (Phillips and Perron) and ADF (Augmented Dickey-Fuller), and insignificant KPSS (Kwiatkowski–Phillips–Schmidt–Shin).
PP | ADF | KPSS | |
EUA | −54.6872*** | −20.0822*** | 0.0477 |
ERIX | −53.9489*** | −25.1472*** | 0.0838 |
Notes: PP, ADF, and KPSS are unit root tests. *** represents the rejection of the null hypothesis of unit root and non-stationarity at 1% significance level. |
We chose the generalized OGARCH model to examine the carbon asset association with other markets, such as the clean energy market. The GO-GARCH model of Van der Weide (2002) is linked with a set of independent univariate and conditionally uncorrelated GARCH processes, which uses marginal density parameters and relate them to the observed data (Ghalanos, 2014) that can offer more flexibility in the estimation, compared to other MGARCH models.
In the GO-GARCH model, the market return is denoted by (rt), the conditional mean (mt) which is a function of (rt) contains error term (εt) and an AR(1) term.
Qt=rt+εt | (1) |
The model draws Qt−rt on a set of unobservable independent factorsft.
εt=Aft | (2) |
where the mixing matrix A is divided into a rotational matrix U and an unconditional covariance matrix. The covariance matrix of rt will then be computed as:
ogt=var(rt)=wtVar(Qt)wTt=wtZtwTt | (3) |
where Zt = diag matrix, with the dependent column variance in Qt represented by its elements modeled by GARCH (1, 1).
Qi,j=vi+εi,j |
Qi,j=vi+εi,jσ2i,j=α0+μ1εi,j−12+μ2σ2i,j−1 | (4) |
where Qi, j is the i-th principle component on j time, vi is a constant, σi, j2 is the conditional variance of εi, j, εi = σi, j µi, j with µi, j ~ N(0, 1) for j = 1, …, T.
Solving for ogt, conditional coefficient between security i and e is given as:
Qi,e=ogtie√(ogtiiogtie) | (5) |
The optimal weights are achieved according to the weights designed by (Jammazi and Reboredo, 2016; Chkili et al., 2016), which uses correlations provided by the bivariate models of DCC, ADCC, and GO-GARCH. The output of the portfolios (II, III, and IV) is contrasted to that of the Portfolio benchmark (I), which consists solely of renewable energy. Portfolio II is designed according to Kroner and Ng (1998), and its construction is as under:
wFt=hSt−hFSthCt−2hFSt+hSt | (6) |
where the conditional variance and covariance on the spot (clean energy) and future (clean energy and carbon market) returns are denoted by hF,t and hSF,t. By construction, the weight of the clean energy stock in the portfolio is equal to (1−wFt).
Portfolio III follows variance minimizing strategy, which means an investor has a long position in spot assets (clean energy stock market) and a short position in futures assets (mixed portfolio), which is given by
βt=hFSthFt | (7) |
Portfolio IV is an equally weighted portfolio that follows DeMiguel et al. (2009) with a good out-of-sample approach.
The risk reduction effectiveness (RE) measured for Portfolios II, III, and IV is determined by the percentage reduction in the variance compared to Portfolio (I). It is represented as follows
RE=1−Var(Pi)Var(I) | (8) |
where i = II, III, IV and the variances in the clean energy and carbon assets portfolios and the variance of the benchmark portfolio (I), respectively, are expressed by Var (Pi) and Var (PI). Higher positive values suggest greater efficacy of risk-reduction.
A portfolio's value-at-risk can be given by considering a certain confidence level (1-p):
VaR(p)=μt−t−1v(p)√ht | (9) |
where μt and ht denote the portfolio's conditional mean and standard deviation, accordingly, and tv-1 (p) signifies the pth quartile of the distribution of t and the degrees of freedom of v.
The GO-GARCH model is compared with DCC and ADCC to estimate volatility and correlation forecasts. The symmetric and asymmetric DCCs are estimated with a multivariate student-t distribution to check non-normality in the distribution and estimate GO-GARCH with a multivariate affine negative inverse Gaussian (MANIG) distribution.
Table 4 shows the estimated parameters of DCC and ADCC. For all stock indices, the conditional mean (μ) for the DCC GARCH model is statistically significant and positive; however, the mean value is greater for ERIX. The estimated coefficients for AR(1) for both DCC and ADCC are statistically negative and insignificant in the case of ERIX, whereas significantly positive for EUA. For both DCC and ADCC models, the coefficient values (ω) of the conditional variance are slightly higher for the clean energy market (ERIX) compared to the carbon market (EUA). The estimated coefficients of the (α) terms representing the long-run volatility persistence are positive and statistically significant in both markets for DCC models; however, for ADCC models, only EUA coefficients are significant. For short-term volatility persistence, the coefficients of (β) are significantly positive in the case of ERIX and EUA for both models.
ERIX | EUA | |
Parameter Estimates of DCC | ||
μ | 0.065336*** | 0.013284*** |
(0.01434) | (0.004765) | |
AR (1) | −0.014715 | 0.04878*** |
(0.014575) | (0.014827) | |
ω | 0.014713*** | 0.000944*** |
(0.004167) | (0.000208) | |
α | 0.086571*** | 0.036735*** |
(0.010249) | (0.002429) | |
β | 0.908122*** | 0.955794*** |
(0.010326) | (0.000661) | |
λ | 8.155709*** | 8.629631*** |
(0.946984) | (1.021479) | |
Parameter Estimates of ADCC | ||
μ | 0.033114** | 0.01379*** |
(0.014424) | (0.00482) | |
AR(1) | −0.009366 | 0.048458*** |
(0.014271) | (0.014808) | |
ω | 0.020955*** | 0.000868*** |
(0.004707) | (0.000204) | |
α | 0.00000 | 0.039941*** |
(0.007577) | (0.00507) | |
β | 0.908073*** | 0.957142*** |
(0.011583) | (0.000532) | |
ϒ | 0.154644*** | -0.007307 |
(0.020096) | (0.007914) | |
λ | 9.74964*** | 8.663788*** |
(1.337822) | (1.027544) | |
Note: ***, **, * shows significance at 1%, 5% and 10% levels. The values in parenthesis are standard errors. |
Moreover, the asymmetric coefficients (ϒ) are statistically significant and positive for the clean energy market, whereas negatively insignificant in the carbon market. These results of (ϒ) for ERIX show that the negative residuals will increase the conditional volatility for this series. The findings of this study are in line with Basher and Sadorsky (2016), which argues that different leverage effects may be seen due to different contract liquidity, arbitrage activities, or asymmetric information. The shape parameters (λ) are equivalent to degrees of freedom (d.f). As the number of d.f. reaches infinity, the shape of the t-distribution changes to normal distribution. The shape parameters for both markets are more than 8 in both models but greater in the case of the ADCC model.
We use the generalized orthogonal GO-GARCH (Generalized Autoregressive Conditional Heteroskedasticity) model to estimate volatility and correlation forecasts. GO-GARCH is used to measure the dependency and the volatility of an asset. In Table 5, it appears that the short-run persistence of volatility (α) alpha is lower than the (β) beta long-term persistence of volatility, which implies that the EUA and ERIX take a long time to get out of the market shocks completely. The combined value of alpha and beta is for EUA and ERIX is near to 1, implying that volatility effects remain in the markets for a long time. The research findings of the GO-GARCH model are consistent with the research findings of the DCC and ADCC models.
F1 (ERIX) | F2 (EUA) | |
Omega | 0.008023 | 0.007469 |
Alpha1 | 0.043078 | 0.080302 |
Beta1 | 0.950262 | 0.913516 |
Skewness | 0.062621 | 0.126950 |
Shape | 0.822276 | 3.197485 |
Note: All specifications in the GO-GARCH model include a constant and an AR(1) term in the mean equation. |
We will compare the clean energy portfolio performance containing carbon assets (Portfolio II, III, IV) with the clean energy stock portfolio not containing any carbon assets (Portfolio I) to assess any value-addition using different portfolio strategies. Thus, the results in table 6 depict risk reduction relative to the returns of the assets. Risk reduction gains and diversification benefits are greater in Portfolio II and IV. Regarding the downside risk measures (value-at-risk reduction at 95% level of confidence), Portfolio II provides the highest VaR reduction followed by Portfolio IV and III. Portfolio (II) based on risk minimization strategy and Portfolio IV with equally allocated weights outperforms the benchmark Portfolio (I) with clean energy stock only.
Portfolio II | Portfolio III | Portfolio IV | |
Risk Reduction | 0.9433 | 0.0192 | 0.7535 |
VaR Reduction | 0.7641 | 0.0124 | 0.5050 |
Note: Table shows risk reduction effectiveness and VaR reduction results. Portfolio I is the benchmark portfolio composed of clean energy stocks; Portfolios II and III are given by Eq. (9) and (10). Portfolio IV has equal weights. |
Thus, one can say that adding carbon assets to the clean energy stock adds significant value to the clean energy stock portfolio and the best optimization strategy is Portfolio II. The results are consistent with Zhang et al. (2017) study, which found that adding carbon assets to financial portfolios yields diversification benefits.
EUA can be considered a turning point in lowering environmental damage (Benz et al., 2006). However, continuing allowance trading is insufficient to meet the emission reduction targets successfully; it is also important to establish risk control strategies for carbon risk (Balcılar et al., 2016). Since most previous studies have concentrated on general co-movements between carbon assets and clean energy stocks, they overlooked the value addition to clean energy stock portfolios by including carbon assets, which creates a gap. We add to the literature using the GO-GARCH model to examine the time-varying relationship between the markets under consideration and then build optimal portfolios to compare the values of clean energy stock portfolios with and without carbon assets. We focus on the EU-ETS and ERIX index, the biggest and most representative markets of the assets in consideration. Our results indicate that the EUA stock is independent of the ERIX index leading to value addition by adding carbon assets to the clean energy stock portfolio. This is an initial study on the impact of the addition of the carbon assets to clean energy stock portfolios while modeling the dependence of the variables through GO-GARCH and considering the risk and downside risk measures for portfolio diversification. The empirical analysis reveals that the EUA prices negatively affect clean energy prices, but this effect is significantly positive with green bonds (Leitao et al., 2021). We build portfolio strategies to assess the impact on the value against the benchmark Portfolio strategy I (clean energy stocks). However, Portfolio II, III, and IV are computed relative to risk-minimizing strategy, variance-minimizing strategy, and equally weighted portfolio. The results indicate that the inclusion of carbon assets to clean energy stocks provides the highest risk reduction for Portfolio II. In particular, carbon assets can decrease portfolio risk and increase portfolio return when added to a clean energy stock portfolio. Moreover, our findings are in line with the studies of Liu et al. (2015), which proposed that the carbon assets are weakly correlated to clean energy stock portfolios.
EUA allowances and clean energy are imperative for lowering carbon emissions on a global scale. However, the relation of carbon assets and clean energy stock remains understudied, as the risk management strategies essential for achieving the carbon emission targets have been consistently overlooked. The present paper is an effort to fill this vacuum by elucidating the impact of adding carbon assets to the clean energy stock portfolios. Upon adding carbon assets to alternate assets (clean energy stock portfolios), the risk and the downside risk both tend to fall, and diversification benefits can be achieved. Thus, establishing that simultaneous investment in carbon assets and clean energy stock portfolio is more profitable for clean investors. Therefore, the results highlight the importance of mixed portfolio construction for diversification purposes.
The study has important implications for investors, policymakers, and portfolio managers. Adequate knowledge about value addition by carbon assets can help investors to make better decisions regarding asset allocation. Thus, investors can maximize their value and protect their investment from downside risk by using the information. For policymakers, this information can assist them in designing incentive strategies to attract investors towards more green investments. As the primary goal of emission trading schemes and clean energy stock is to reduce GHG emissions, these policies can provide opportunities for shifting towards green technology. Furthermore, the results allow portfolio managers to devise diversification strategies based on mixed portfolios.
Moreover, this paper is limited to only the European carbon and clean energy market; however, it can be extended to Chinese emerging carbon markets and the clean energy sector. Global and sectoral clean energy indices can also be considered. Other than emission trading schemes, another way of pricing carbon to reduce GHG is to apply a carbon tax due to its simplicity, scope, compatibility with low-carbon transition, and cost-effectiveness (O'Mahony, 2020). Advanced methodologies such as copula and switching copula can be applied to measure the dependence patterns of these markets in different regimes.
All authors declare no conflicts of interest in this paper.
[1] | Ramon Y, Cajal S (1899) Textura del Sistema Nervioso del Hombre y de los Vertebrados trans Madrid: Nicolas Moya. |
[2] | Brodmann K (1909) Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues Leipzig: Johann Ambrosius Barth. |
[3] |
DeFelipe J, Alonso-Nanclares L, Arellano JI (2002) Microstructure of the neocortex: comparative aspects. J Neurocytol 31: 299-316. doi: 10.1023/A:1024130211265
![]() |
[4] |
Jacobs B, Scheibel AB (2002) Regional dendritic variation in primate cortical pyramidal cells. Cortical Areas: Unity and Diversity New York: CRC Press, 111-131. doi: 10.1201/9780203299296.pt2
![]() |
[5] |
Elston GN (2007) Specialization of the Neocortical Pyramidal Cell during Primate Evolution. Evolution of Nervous Systems, Volume IV Oxford: Academic Press, 191-242. doi: 10.1016/B0-12-370878-8/00164-6
![]() |
[6] |
Elston GN, Garey LJ (2013) The cytoarchitectonic map of Korbinian Brodmann: arealisation and circuit specialisation. Microstructural Parcellation of the Human Cerebral Cortex Berlin Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, 3-32. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-45766-5_1
![]() |
[7] |
Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, et al. (2005) Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature 436: 801-806. doi: 10.1038/nature03721
![]() |
[8] |
Freiwald WA, Tsao DY (2010) Functional compartmentalization and viewpoint generalization within the macaque face-processing system. Science 330: 845-851. doi: 10.1126/science.1194908
![]() |
[9] |
DiCarlo JJ, Zoccolan D, Rust NC (2012) How does the brain solve visual object recognition? Neuron 73: 415-434. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.01.010
![]() |
[10] |
Kriegeskorte N, Kievit RA (2013) Representational geometry: integrating cognition, computation, and the brain. Trends Cognit Sci 17: 401-412. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.007
![]() |
[11] |
Constantinescu AO, O'Reilly JX, Behrens TE (2016) Organizing conceptual knowledge in humans with a gridlike code. Science 352: 1464-1468. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf0941
![]() |
[12] |
Fusi S, Miller EK, Rigotti M (2016) Why neurons mix: high dimensionality for higher cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol 37: 66-74. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.01.010
![]() |
[13] |
Rolls ET (2016) Cerebral Cortex: Principles of Operation Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198784852.001.0001
![]() |
[14] |
Chang L, Tsao DY (2017) The code for facial identity in the primate brain. Cell 169: 1013-1028. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.011
![]() |
[15] |
Diedrichsen J, Kriegeskorte N (2017) Representational models: A common framework for understanding encoding, pattern-component, and representational-similarity analysis. PLOS Comput Biol 13: e1005508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005508
![]() |
[16] |
Landi SM, Freiwald WA (2017) Two areas for familiar face recognition in the primate brain. Science 357: 591-595. doi: 10.1126/science.aan1139
![]() |
[17] |
Kriegeskorte N, Diedrichsen J (2019) Peeling the onion of brain representations. Annu Rev Neurosci 42: 407-432. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-061906
![]() |
[18] |
Hinton GE (1989) Connectionist learning procedures. Artif Intell 40: 185-234. doi: 10.1016/0004-3702(89)90049-0
![]() |
[19] |
Schmidhuber J (2015) Deep learning in neural networks: An overview. Neural Netw 61: 85-117. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2014.09.003
![]() |
[20] |
Marblestone AH, Wayne G, Kording KP (2016) Toward an integration of deep learning and neuroscience. Front Comput Neurosci 10: 94. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2016.00094
![]() |
[21] |
Yamins DL, DiCarlo JJ (2016) Using goal-driven deep learning models to understand sensory cortex. Nat Neurosci 19: 356-365. doi: 10.1038/nn.4244
![]() |
[22] |
Beyeler M, Rounds EL, Carlson KD, et al. (2019) Neural correlates of sparse coding and dimensionality reduction. PLOS Comput Biol 15: e1006908. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006908
![]() |
[23] |
Yang GR, Joglekar MR, Song HF, et al. (2019) Task representations in neural networks trained to perform many cognitive tasks. Nat Neurosci 22: 297-306. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0310-2
![]() |
[24] |
Ahissar M, Hochstein S (1993) Attentional control of early perceptual learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90: 5718-5722. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.12.5718
![]() |
[25] | Adelson EH (2000) Lightness Perception and Lightness Illusions. The New Cognitive Neurosciences Cambridge: The MIT Press, 339-351. |
[26] |
Fahle M (2004) Perceptual learning: A case for early selection. J Vis 4: 879-890. doi: 10.1167/4.10.4
![]() |
[27] |
Song Y, Ding Y, Fan S, et al. (2005) Neural substrates of visual perceptual learning of simple and complex stimuli. Clin Neurophysiol 116: 632-639. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.09.019
![]() |
[28] |
Sasaki Y, Nanez JE, Watanabe T (2010) Advances in visual perceptual learning and plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci 11: 53-60. doi: 10.1038/nrn2737
![]() |
[29] |
Koster-Hale J, Saxe R (2013) Theory of mind: a neural prediction problem. Neuron 79: 836-848. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.020
![]() |
[30] |
Larcombe SJ, Kennard C, Bridge H (2017) Time course influences transfer of visual perceptual learning across spatial location. Vision Res 135: 26-33. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2017.04.002
![]() |
[31] |
Buzsaki G, Moser EI (2013) Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat Neurosci 16: 130-138. doi: 10.1038/nn.3304
![]() |
[32] |
Rigotti M, Barak O, Warden MR, et al. (2013) The importance of mixed selectivity in complex cognitive tasks. Nature 497: 585-590. doi: 10.1038/nature12160
![]() |
[33] |
Moser MB, Rowland DC, Moser EI (2015) Place cells, grid cells, and memory. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 7: a021808. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021808
![]() |
[34] |
Saxena S, Cunningham JP (2019) Towards the neural population doctrine. Curr Opin Neurobiol 55: 103-111. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2019.02.002
![]() |
[35] |
Prasad S, Galetta SL (2011) Anatomy and physiology of the afferent visual system. Handbook of Clinical Neurology Elsevier, 3-19. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-52903-9.00007-8
![]() |
[36] |
De Sousa AA, Proulx MJ (2014) What can volumes reveal about human brain evolution? A framework for bridging behavioral, histometric, and volumetric perspectives. Front Neuroanat 8: 51. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2014.00051
![]() |
[37] |
Recanzone GH, Schreiner CE, Merzenich MM (1993) Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination training in adult owl monkeys. J Neurosci 3: 87-103. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-01-00087.1993
![]() |
[38] |
Wang X, Merzenich MM (1995) Remodelling of hand representation in adult. Nature 378: 2. doi: 10.1038/378071a0
![]() |
[39] |
Wallis G, Rolls ET (1997) Invariant face and object recognition in the visual system. Prog Neurobiol 51: 167-194. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(96)00054-8
![]() |
[40] |
Yang Z, Purves D (2004) The statistical structure of natural light patterns determines perceived light intensity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 8745-8750. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402192101
![]() |
[41] |
Smithson HE (2005) Sensory, computational and cognitive components of human colour constancy. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360: 1329-1346. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1633
![]() |
[42] |
Corney D, Lotto RB (2007) What are lightness illusions and why do we see them? PLOS Comput Biol 3: 1790-1800. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030180
![]() |
[43] |
Garrigan P, Kellman PJ (2008) Perceptual learning depends on perceptual constancy. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105: 2248-2253. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711878105
![]() |
[44] |
Werner A (2014) Spatial and temporal aspects of chromatic adaptation and their functional significance for colour constancy. Vision Res 104: 80-89. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.10.005
![]() |
[45] |
Kravitz DJ, Saleem KS, Baker CI, et al. (2013) The ventral visual pathway: an expanded neural framework for the processing of object quality. Trends Cognit Sci 17: 26-49. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.011
![]() |
[46] |
Cichy RM, Pantazis D, Oliva A (2014) Resolving human object recognition in space and time. Nat Neurosci 17: 455-462. doi: 10.1038/nn.3635
![]() |
[47] |
Geskin J, Behrmann M (2018) Congenital prosopagnosia without object agnosia? A literature review. Cognit Neuropsychol 35: 4-54. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2017.1392295
![]() |
[48] |
De Beeck HP, Pillet I, Ritchie JB (2019) Factors determining where category-selective areas emerge in visual cortex. Trends Cognit Sci 23: 784-797. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.06.006
![]() |
[49] |
Kar K, Kubilius J, Schmidt K, et al. (2019) Evidence that recurrent circuits are critical to the ventral stream's execution of core object recognition behavior. Nat Neurosci 22: 974-983. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0392-5
![]() |
[50] |
Eichenbaum H, Cohen NJ (2014) Can we reconcile the declarative memory and spatial navigation views on hippocampal function? Neuron 83: 764-770. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.032
![]() |
[51] |
Rolls ET, Wirth S (2018) Spatial representations in the primate hippocampus, and their functions in memory and navigation. Prog Neurobiol 171: 90-113. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2018.09.004
![]() |
[52] |
Tsutsui KI, Sakata H, Naganuma T, et al. (2002) Neural correlates for perception of 3D surface orientation from texture gradient. Science 298: 409-412. doi: 10.1126/science.1074128
![]() |
[53] |
Matthews N, Meng X, Xu P, et al. (2003) A physiological theory of depth perception from vertical disparity. Vision Res 43: 85-99. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00401-7
![]() |
[54] |
Liu Y, Vogels R, Orban GA (2004) Convergence of depth from texture and depth from disparity in macaque inferior temporal cortex. J Neurosci 24: 3795-3800. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0150-04.2004
![]() |
[55] |
Slater A, Mattock A, Brown E (1990) Size constancy at birth: Newborn infants' responses to retinal and real size. J Exp Child Psychol 49: 314-322. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(90)90061-C
![]() |
[56] |
Liu Z (1999) Perceptual learning in motion discrimination that generalizes across motion directions. Pro Natl Acad Sci USA 96: 14085-14087. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.24.14085
![]() |
[57] |
Sikl R, Simecek M, Porubanova-Norquist M, et al. (2013) Vision after 53 years of blindness. i-Percept 4: 498-507. doi: 10.1068/i0611
![]() |
[58] |
Lui JH, Hansen DV, Kriegstein AR (2011) Development and evolution of the human neocortex. Cell 146: 18-36. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.06.030
![]() |
[59] | Fowler AE (1991) How Early Phonological Development Might Set the Stage for Phoneme Awareness. Phonological Processes in Literacy: A Tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 97-117. |
[60] |
Kuhl PK, Williams KA, Lacerda F, et al. (1992) Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science 255: 606-608. doi: 10.1126/science.1736364
![]() |
[61] |
Cheour M, Ceponiene R, Lehtokoski A, et al. (1998) Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain. Nat Neurosci 1: 351-353. doi: 10.1038/1561
![]() |
[62] |
Cowey A, Rolls ET (1974) Human cortical magnification factor and its relation to visual acuity. Exp Brain Res 21: 447-454. doi: 10.1007/BF00237163
![]() |
[63] |
Shum J, Hermes D, Foster BL, et al. (2013) A brain area for visual numerals. J Neurosci 33: 6709-6715. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4558-12.2013
![]() |
[64] |
Blumberg J, Kreiman G (2010) How cortical neurons help us see: visual recognition in the human brain. J Clin Invest 120: 3054-3063. doi: 10.1172/JCI42161
![]() |
[65] |
Rolls ET, Baylis GC (1986) Size and contrast have only small effects on the responses to faces of neurons in the cortex of the superior temporal sulcus of the monkey. Exp Brain Res 65: 38-48. doi: 10.1007/BF00243828
![]() |
[66] |
Sharma J, Angelucci A, Sur M (2000) Induction of visual orientation modules in auditory cortex. Nature 404: 841-847. doi: 10.1038/35009043
![]() |
[67] |
Von Melchner L, Pallas SL, Sur M (2000) Visual behaviour mediated by retinal projections directed to the auditory pathway. Nature 404: 871-876. doi: 10.1038/35009102
![]() |
[68] | Simon HA (1962) The architecture of complexity. Proc Am Philos Soc 106: 467-482. |
[69] |
Meunier D, Lambiotte R, Bullmore ET (2010) Modular and hierarchically modular organization of brain networks. Front Neurosci 4: 200. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2010.00200
![]() |
[70] |
Muckli L, Naumer MJ, Singer W (2009) Bilateral visual field maps in a patient with only one hemisphere. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 13034-13039. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809688106
![]() |
[71] |
Borst A, Helmstaedter M (2015) Common circuit design in fly and mammalian motion vision. Nat Neurosci 18: 1067-1076. doi: 10.1038/nn.4050
![]() |
[72] |
De Andres-Bragado L, Sprecher SG (2019) Mechanisms of vision in the fruit fly. Curr Opin Insect Sci 36: 25-32. doi: 10.1016/j.cois.2019.06.005
![]() |
[73] |
Lee YJ, Nordstrom K (2012) Higher-order motion sensitivity in fly visual circuits. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109: 8758-8763. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1203081109
![]() |
[74] |
Tanaka K (1996) Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annu Rev Neurosci 19: 109-139. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.19.030196.000545
![]() |
[75] |
Hung CP, Kreiman G, Poggio T, et al. (2005) Fast readout of object identity from macaque inferior temporal cortex. Science 310: 863-866. doi: 10.1126/science.1117593
![]() |
[76] |
Kiani R, Esteky H, Mirpour K, et al. (2007) Object category structure in response patterns of neuronal population in monkey inferior temporal cortex. J Neurophysiol 97: 4296-4309. doi: 10.1152/jn.00024.2007
![]() |
[77] |
Yamins DL, Hong H, Cadieu CF, et al. (2014) Performance-optimized hierarchical models predict neural responses in higher visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111: 8619-8624. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1403112111
![]() |
[78] |
Clarke A, Tyler LK (2015) Understanding what we see: how we derive meaning from vision. Trends Cognit Sci 19: 677-687. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.008
![]() |
[79] |
Kohn A (2007) Visual adaptation: physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. J Neurophysiol 97: 3155-3164. doi: 10.1152/jn.00086.2007
![]() |
[80] |
Ress D, Heeger DJ (2003) Neuronal correlates of perception in early visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 6: 414-420. doi: 10.1038/nn1024
![]() |
[81] |
Jancke D, Chavane F, Naaman S, et al. (2004) Imaging cortical correlates of illusion in early visual cortex. Nature 428: 423-426. doi: 10.1038/nature02396
![]() |
[82] | O'Keefe J, Nadel L (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map Oxford: Clarendon Press. |
[83] |
Brown MW, Aggleton JP (2001) Recognition memory: what are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 51-61. doi: 10.1038/35049064
![]() |
[84] |
Eichenbaum H (2004) Hippocampus: cognitive processes and neural representations that underlie declarative memory. Neuron 44: 109-120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.028
![]() |
[85] |
Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK, Barnes CA, et al. (2005) Independent codes for spatial and episodic memory in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Science 309: 619-623. doi: 10.1126/science.1114037
![]() |
[86] |
Konkel A, Cohen NJ (2009) Relational memory and the hippocampus: representations and methods. Front Neurosci 3: 166-174. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.023.2009
![]() |
[87] |
Bassett DS, Bullmore ED (2006) Small-world brain networks. Neuroscientist 12: 512-523. doi: 10.1177/1073858406293182
![]() |
[88] |
Kanwisher N, Wojciulik E (2000) Visual attention: insights from brain imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 1: 91-100. doi: 10.1038/35039043
![]() |
[89] |
Ungerleider SK (2000) Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 23: 315-341. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315
![]() |
[90] |
Itti L, Koch C (2001) Computational modelling of visual attention. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 194-203. doi: 10.1038/35058500
![]() |
[91] |
Lamme VA (2003) Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends Cognit Sci 7: 12-18. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-X
![]() |
[92] |
Serences JT, Yantis S (2006) Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence. Trends Cognit Sci 10: 38-45. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.008
![]() |
[93] |
Mack ML, Love BC, Preston AR (2016) Dynamic updating of hippocampal object representations reflects new conceptual knowledge. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113: 13203-13208. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1614048113
![]() |
[94] |
White BJ, Berg DJ, Kan JY, et al. (2017) Superior colliculus neurons encode a visual saliency map during free viewing of natural dynamic video. Nat Commun 8: 1-9. doi: 10.1038/s41467-016-0009-6
![]() |
[95] |
Rademaker RL, Chunharas C, Serences JT (2019) Coexisting representations of sensory and mnemonic information in human visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 22: 1336-1344. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0428-x
![]() |
[96] |
Pang R, Lansdell BJ, Fairhall AL (2016) Dimensionality reduction in neuroscience. Curr Biol 26: R656-R660. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.029
![]() |
[97] | Gosmann J, Eliasmith C (2020) CUE: A unified spiking neuron model of short-term and long-term memory. Psychol Rev Available from: https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000250. |
[98] |
Vyas S, Golub MD, Sussillo D, et al. (2020) Computation Through Neural Population Dynamics. Annu Rev Neurosci 43: 249-275. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-092619-094115
![]() |
[99] |
Leskovec J, Sosic R (2016) Snap: A general-purpose network analysis and graph-mining library. ACM Trans Intell Syst Technol 8: 1-20. doi: 10.1145/2898361
![]() |
[100] |
Betzel RF, Bassett DS (2017) Generative models for network neuroscience: prospects and promise. J R Soc Interface 14: 20170623. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2017.0623
![]() |
[101] |
Guest O, Love BC (2017) What the success of brain imaging implies about the neural code. Elife 6: e21397. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21397
![]() |
[102] |
Avena-Koenigsberger A, Misic B, Sporns O (2018) Communication dynamics in complex brain networks. Nat Rev Neurosci 19: 17-33. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.149
![]() |
[103] |
Koutrouli M, Karatzas E, Paez-Espino D, et al. (2020) A Guide to Conquer the Biological Network Era Using Graph Theory. Front Bioeng Biotechnol 8: 34. doi: 10.3389/fbioe.2020.00034
![]() |
[104] |
Kriegeskorte N (2015) Deep neural networks: a new framework for modeling biological vision and brain information processing. Annu Rev Vis Sci 1: 417-446. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035447
![]() |
[105] |
Kell AJ, Yamins DL, Shook EN, et al. (2018) A task-optimized neural network replicates human auditory behavior, predicts brain responses, and reveals a cortical processing hierarchy. Neuron 98: 630-644. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.044
![]() |
[106] |
Bashivan P, Kar K, DiCarlo JJ (2019) Neural population control via deep image synthesis. Science 364: eaav9436. doi: 10.1126/science.aav9436
![]() |
[107] |
Stringer C, Pachitariu M, Steinmetz N, et al. (2019) High-dimensional geometry of population responses in visual cortex. Nature 571: 361-365. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1346-5
![]() |
[108] |
Atick JJ, Redlich AN (1990) Towards a theory of early visual processing. Neural Comput 2: 308-320. doi: 10.1162/neco.1990.2.3.308
![]() |
[109] |
Dan Y, Atick JJ, Reid RC (1996) Efficient coding of natural scenes in the lateral geniculate nucleus: experimental test of a computational theory. J Neurosci 16: 3351-3362. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-10-03351.1996
![]() |
[110] |
Doi E, Gauthier JL, Field GD, et al. (2012) Efficient coding of spatial information in the primate retina. J Neurosci 32: 16256-16264. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4036-12.2012
![]() |
[111] |
Sporns O (2011) The non-random brain: efficiency, economy, and complex dynamics. Front Comput Neurosci 5: 5. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2011.00005
![]() |
[112] |
Smith EH, Horga G, Yates MJ, et al. (2019) Widespread temporal coding of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 22: 1883-1891. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0494-0
![]() |
[113] |
Yates AJ (1963) Delayed auditory feedback. Psychol Bull 60: 213-232. doi: 10.1037/h0044155
![]() |
[114] |
Marin OS, Perry DW (1999) Neurological aspects of music perception and performance. The psychology of music Academic Press, 653-724. doi: 10.1016/B978-012213564-4/50018-4
![]() |
[115] |
Barnes GR (2008) Cognitive processes involved in smooth pursuit eye movements. Brain Cogn 68: 309-326. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.020
![]() |
[116] |
David N, Newen A, Vogeley K (2008) The “sense of agency” and its underlying cognitive and neural mechanisms. Conscious Cognit 17: 523-534. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.004
![]() |
[117] |
Rohrmeier MA, Koelsch S (2012) Predictive information processing in music cognition. A critical review. Int J Psychophysiol 83: 164-175. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.12.010
![]() |
[118] |
Rohde M, Van Dam LC, Ernst MO (2014) Predictability is necessary for closed-loop visual feedback delay adaptation. J Vision 14: 4. doi: 10.1167/14.3.4
![]() |
[119] |
Okada K, Matchin W, Hickok G (2018) Neural evidence for predictive coding in auditory cortex during speech production. Psychon Bull Rev 25: 423-430. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1284-x
![]() |
[120] |
Lyu B, Choi HS, Marslen-Wilson WD, et al. (2019) Neural dynamics of semantic composition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 116: 21318-21327. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1903402116
![]() |
[121] |
Libet B, Gleason CA, Wright EW, et al. (1993) Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). Neurophysiology of Consciousness Boston: Birkhäuser, 249-268. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4612-0355-1_15
![]() |
[122] |
Haggard P, Clark S (2003) Intentional action: Conscious experience and neural prediction. Conscious Cogn 12: 695-707. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00052-7
![]() |
[123] |
Haggard P (2005) Conscious intention and motor cognition. Trends Cognit Sci 9: 290-295. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.012
![]() |
[124] |
Moore JW, Wegner DM, Haggard P (2009) Modulating the sense of agency with external cues. Conscious Cogn 18: 1056-1064. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.05.004
![]() |
[125] |
Moore JW, Obhi SS (2012) Intentional binding and the sense of agency: a review. Conscious Cogn 21: 546-561. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.12.002
![]() |
[126] |
Wen W, Yamashita A, Asama H (2015) The influence of action-outcome delay and arousal on sense of agency and the intentional binding effect. Conscious Cogn 36: 87-95. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.06.004
![]() |
[127] |
Haggard P (2017) Sense of agency in the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 18: 196-207. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.14
![]() |
[128] |
Wegner DM (2002) The illusion of conscious will The MIT press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/3650.001.0001
![]() |
[129] |
Komer B, Eliasmith C (2016) A unified theoretical approach for biological cognition and learning. Curr Opin Behav Sci 11: 14-20. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.03.006
![]() |
[130] |
Bullmore E, Sporns O (2012) The economy of brain network organization. Nat Rev Neurosci 13: 336-349. doi: 10.1038/nrn3214
![]() |
[131] |
Stiles J, Jernigan TL (2010) The basics of brain development. Neuropsychol Rev 20: 327-348. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9148-4
![]() |
[132] |
Budday S, Steinmann P, Kuhl E (2015) Physical biology of human brain development. Front Cell Neurosci 9: 257. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00257
![]() |
EUA | ERIX | |
Min | −0.4321 | −0.1297 |
Max | 0.2382 | 0.0795 |
Mean | 0.0067 | 0.0033 |
Std. Dev. | 0.0316 | 0.0164 |
Skewness | −0.8162 | −0.3595 |
Kurtosis | 17.7436 | 6.2519 |
ERIX | EUA | |
ERIX | 1 | |
EUA | −0.40* | 1 |
Notes: *Represents significance at 10% level. |
PP | ADF | KPSS | |
EUA | −54.6872*** | −20.0822*** | 0.0477 |
ERIX | −53.9489*** | −25.1472*** | 0.0838 |
Notes: PP, ADF, and KPSS are unit root tests. *** represents the rejection of the null hypothesis of unit root and non-stationarity at 1% significance level. |
ERIX | EUA | |
Parameter Estimates of DCC | ||
μ | 0.065336*** | 0.013284*** |
(0.01434) | (0.004765) | |
AR (1) | −0.014715 | 0.04878*** |
(0.014575) | (0.014827) | |
ω | 0.014713*** | 0.000944*** |
(0.004167) | (0.000208) | |
α | 0.086571*** | 0.036735*** |
(0.010249) | (0.002429) | |
β | 0.908122*** | 0.955794*** |
(0.010326) | (0.000661) | |
λ | 8.155709*** | 8.629631*** |
(0.946984) | (1.021479) | |
Parameter Estimates of ADCC | ||
μ | 0.033114** | 0.01379*** |
(0.014424) | (0.00482) | |
AR(1) | −0.009366 | 0.048458*** |
(0.014271) | (0.014808) | |
ω | 0.020955*** | 0.000868*** |
(0.004707) | (0.000204) | |
α | 0.00000 | 0.039941*** |
(0.007577) | (0.00507) | |
β | 0.908073*** | 0.957142*** |
(0.011583) | (0.000532) | |
ϒ | 0.154644*** | -0.007307 |
(0.020096) | (0.007914) | |
λ | 9.74964*** | 8.663788*** |
(1.337822) | (1.027544) | |
Note: ***, **, * shows significance at 1%, 5% and 10% levels. The values in parenthesis are standard errors. |
F1 (ERIX) | F2 (EUA) | |
Omega | 0.008023 | 0.007469 |
Alpha1 | 0.043078 | 0.080302 |
Beta1 | 0.950262 | 0.913516 |
Skewness | 0.062621 | 0.126950 |
Shape | 0.822276 | 3.197485 |
Note: All specifications in the GO-GARCH model include a constant and an AR(1) term in the mean equation. |
Portfolio II | Portfolio III | Portfolio IV | |
Risk Reduction | 0.9433 | 0.0192 | 0.7535 |
VaR Reduction | 0.7641 | 0.0124 | 0.5050 |
Note: Table shows risk reduction effectiveness and VaR reduction results. Portfolio I is the benchmark portfolio composed of clean energy stocks; Portfolios II and III are given by Eq. (9) and (10). Portfolio IV has equal weights. |
EUA | ERIX | |
Min | −0.4321 | −0.1297 |
Max | 0.2382 | 0.0795 |
Mean | 0.0067 | 0.0033 |
Std. Dev. | 0.0316 | 0.0164 |
Skewness | −0.8162 | −0.3595 |
Kurtosis | 17.7436 | 6.2519 |
ERIX | EUA | |
ERIX | 1 | |
EUA | −0.40* | 1 |
Notes: *Represents significance at 10% level. |
PP | ADF | KPSS | |
EUA | −54.6872*** | −20.0822*** | 0.0477 |
ERIX | −53.9489*** | −25.1472*** | 0.0838 |
Notes: PP, ADF, and KPSS are unit root tests. *** represents the rejection of the null hypothesis of unit root and non-stationarity at 1% significance level. |
ERIX | EUA | |
Parameter Estimates of DCC | ||
μ | 0.065336*** | 0.013284*** |
(0.01434) | (0.004765) | |
AR (1) | −0.014715 | 0.04878*** |
(0.014575) | (0.014827) | |
ω | 0.014713*** | 0.000944*** |
(0.004167) | (0.000208) | |
α | 0.086571*** | 0.036735*** |
(0.010249) | (0.002429) | |
β | 0.908122*** | 0.955794*** |
(0.010326) | (0.000661) | |
λ | 8.155709*** | 8.629631*** |
(0.946984) | (1.021479) | |
Parameter Estimates of ADCC | ||
μ | 0.033114** | 0.01379*** |
(0.014424) | (0.00482) | |
AR(1) | −0.009366 | 0.048458*** |
(0.014271) | (0.014808) | |
ω | 0.020955*** | 0.000868*** |
(0.004707) | (0.000204) | |
α | 0.00000 | 0.039941*** |
(0.007577) | (0.00507) | |
β | 0.908073*** | 0.957142*** |
(0.011583) | (0.000532) | |
ϒ | 0.154644*** | -0.007307 |
(0.020096) | (0.007914) | |
λ | 9.74964*** | 8.663788*** |
(1.337822) | (1.027544) | |
Note: ***, **, * shows significance at 1%, 5% and 10% levels. The values in parenthesis are standard errors. |
F1 (ERIX) | F2 (EUA) | |
Omega | 0.008023 | 0.007469 |
Alpha1 | 0.043078 | 0.080302 |
Beta1 | 0.950262 | 0.913516 |
Skewness | 0.062621 | 0.126950 |
Shape | 0.822276 | 3.197485 |
Note: All specifications in the GO-GARCH model include a constant and an AR(1) term in the mean equation. |
Portfolio II | Portfolio III | Portfolio IV | |
Risk Reduction | 0.9433 | 0.0192 | 0.7535 |
VaR Reduction | 0.7641 | 0.0124 | 0.5050 |
Note: Table shows risk reduction effectiveness and VaR reduction results. Portfolio I is the benchmark portfolio composed of clean energy stocks; Portfolios II and III are given by Eq. (9) and (10). Portfolio IV has equal weights. |