Errors can affect our memory, yet even when there are gaps in our recollection of events, memory often serves us fairly well. Memory formation involves at least three different sub-processes, that are regulated by an underlying neural structure. From a cognitive neuropsychological perspective, a complex process of encoding, consolidating, and retrieval is involved in remembering an event, and it might be hindered by one's emotional state, physiological response to the event itself, and misinformation. As a result, it is very likely that one may struggle to remember specifics of what happened which can increase our susceptibility to the formation of false memories. This has major implications for everyday functioning, as in the case when you mistakenly remember you took your pills when you never did, or where errors have led to false accusations about trauma or abuse, and wrongful convictions of crimes. Memories sometimes contain biases and inaccuracies that prevent them from accurately recalling events. The review will provide an updated overview of current research advances on the cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying inaccurate, distorted, or false memories.
Citation: Antonio G. Lentoor. Cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying false memories: misinformation, distortion or erroneous configuration?[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2023, 10(3): 255-268. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2023020
Related Papers:
[1]
Xinran Zhou, Long Zhang, Tao Zheng, Hong-li Li, Zhidong Teng .
Global stability for a class of HIV virus-to-cell dynamical model with Beddington-DeAngelis functional response and distributed time delay. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2020, 17(5): 4527-4543.
doi: 10.3934/mbe.2020250
[2]
Renji Han, Binxiang Dai, Lin Wang .
Delay induced spatiotemporal patterns in a diffusive intraguild predation model with Beddington-DeAngelis functional response. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2018, 15(3): 595-627.
doi: 10.3934/mbe.2018027
[3]
Yu Yang, Gang Huang, Yueping Dong .
Stability and Hopf bifurcation of an HIV infection model with two time delays. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2023, 20(2): 1938-1959.
doi: 10.3934/mbe.2023089
[4]
A. M. Elaiw, A. S. Shflot, A. D. Hobiny .
Stability analysis of general delayed HTLV-I dynamics model with mitosis and CTL immunity. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2022, 19(12): 12693-12729.
doi: 10.3934/mbe.2022593
[5]
Cameron Browne .
Immune response in virus model structured by cell infection-age. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2016, 13(5): 887-909.
doi: 10.3934/mbe.2016022
[6]
Ting Guo, Zhipeng Qiu .
The effects of CTL immune response on HIV infection model with potent therapy, latently infected cells and cell-to-cell viral transmission. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2019, 16(6): 6822-6841.
doi: 10.3934/mbe.2019341
[7]
Jinhu Xu, Yicang Zhou .
Bifurcation analysis of HIV-1 infection model with cell-to-cell transmission and immune response delay. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2016, 13(2): 343-367.
doi: 10.3934/mbe.2015006
[8]
Ning Bai, Rui Xu .
Mathematical analysis of an HIV model with latent reservoir, delayed CTL immune response and immune impairment. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2021, 18(2): 1689-1707.
doi: 10.3934/mbe.2021087
[9]
Xin-You Meng, Yu-Qian Wu .
Bifurcation analysis in a singular Beddington-DeAngelis predator-prey model with two delays and nonlinear predator harvesting. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2019, 16(4): 2668-2696.
doi: 10.3934/mbe.2019133
[10]
Jiawei Deng, Ping Jiang, Hongying Shu .
Viral infection dynamics with mitosis, intracellular delays and immune response. Mathematical Biosciences and Engineering, 2023, 20(2): 2937-2963.
doi: 10.3934/mbe.2023139
Abstract
Errors can affect our memory, yet even when there are gaps in our recollection of events, memory often serves us fairly well. Memory formation involves at least three different sub-processes, that are regulated by an underlying neural structure. From a cognitive neuropsychological perspective, a complex process of encoding, consolidating, and retrieval is involved in remembering an event, and it might be hindered by one's emotional state, physiological response to the event itself, and misinformation. As a result, it is very likely that one may struggle to remember specifics of what happened which can increase our susceptibility to the formation of false memories. This has major implications for everyday functioning, as in the case when you mistakenly remember you took your pills when you never did, or where errors have led to false accusations about trauma or abuse, and wrongful convictions of crimes. Memories sometimes contain biases and inaccuracies that prevent them from accurately recalling events. The review will provide an updated overview of current research advances on the cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying inaccurate, distorted, or false memories.
Abbreviations
fMRI:
functional magnetic resonance imaging;
iEEG:
intracranial electroencephalogram;
ACC:
anterior cingulate cortex;
vmPFC:
ventromedial prefrontal cortex;
CMS:
cortical midline structures;
a/dlPFC:
anterior/dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
1.
Introduction
Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) mainly infects CD4+ T-cells in human body and destroys the immune system gradually, which leads to the occurrence of various opportunistic infections. HIV can persist in a latent form in resting CD4+ T-cells, and it can give rise to infectious virus upon stimulation in vitro, which becomes an obstacle to the eradication of virus [1,2]. The mathematical model of HIV infection has attracted more and more scholars' attention, and the classical virus dynamics model is composed of three populations: healthy T-cells, infected T-cells and virus [3,4]. Recently, the four-dimensional mathematical models incorporating latent infection have been developed to describe the mechanism of the latency [5,6].
When HIV invades the human body, the human body makes two modes of immune responses: one is the humoral immune response mediated by B-cell, and the other is the cellular immune response mediated by cytotoxic T lymphocyte (CTL). It is not clear which immune response mode is more effective. Previous studies have established that the humoral immune response is more effective than the cellular immune reponse in malaria infection [7]. Therefore, we focus on the humoral immune response mediated by B-cell here.
In the process of virus infection, we are more concerned about the number of new infectious virions in unit time, that is, the incidence rate. The incidence rate function of virus infection model is mostly bilinear βTV, and then gradually develops to saturation function βTV1+aT[8], Holling type II function βTV1+bV[9] and Beddington-DeAngelis function βTV1+aT+bV[10,11,12] (β is the infection rate, and a,b≥0 are the inhibition constants). The Beddington-DeAngelis function is firstly proposed by Beddington [14] and DeAngelis et al. [15], and it is a generalized infection function, which includes the cases of bilinear, saturation and Holling type II functions. Hence, we will consider the Beddington-DeAngelis incidence rate.
In the process of HIV infection, it takes time for the virus to infect healthy T-cells and then release infectious virus particles. Herz et al. first introduced an intracellular delay to reflect this time period and showed that the model with time delay could shorten the half-life of free virus [16]. Since then, many researchers have analyzed the effect of intracellular delay on the stability of the equilibrium of the virus infection models. Their results showed that the intracellular delay could prolong the progress of the virus, but did not affect the stability of the equilibrium essentially [9,11,12,13,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28]. In addition, it also takes time for antigenic stimulation to generate an immune response [29]. It shows that the immune delay makes the positive equilibrium unstable and leads to Hopf bifurcation. So, both the intracellular delay and the immune delay will be included in our model, which can reflect the progress of virus more practically.
Motivated by the works of [17,18,19], we propose an HIV virus infection model incorporating the latent infection, the Beddington-DeAngelis function infection rate, the humoral immunity and mutiple time delays, which can be described as
Here, T(t), L(t), I(t), V(t) and B(t) denote the number of healthy T-cells, latently infected T-cells, actively infected T-cells, virions and B-cells at time t, respectively. The healthy T-cells are assumed to be input with a constant rate λ, and it can be infected by the virus with the Beddington-DeAngelis functional response βT(t)V(t)1+aT(t)+bV(t). The constant η is the fraction of the infected T-cells leading to latency. Latently infected T-cells can be activated and then becomes the productively infected T-cells with a constant α. Parameter k is the production rate of each infected T-cell, and di(i=1,2,3,4,5) is the death rate of each population. p and q are the B-cell effectiveness and responsiveness, respectively. Here, we consider four time delays: (ⅰ) τ1 represents a time delay between the initial virus entering into the cell and the subsequent viral latency; (ⅱ) τ2 represents a time delay between the cell infection and the subsequent viral generation; (ⅲ) τ3 is the time it takes from the newly produced virus to be mature and then infectious; (ⅳ) τ4 is the time for the B-cell immune system to be activated. Factors e−mτ1 and e−mτ2 denote the probability that an infected T-cell survives the interval τ1 and τ2, respectively.
The rest of this paper is arranged as follows. In section 2, we analyze the well-posedness of the model solution, and obtain the basic reproduction number, the immune response reproduction number and three equilibria. In section 3, by analyzing the characteristic equation at each equilibrium and employing appropriate Lyapunov functions, the global asymptotic stability of uninfected, B-cell-inactivated and B-cell-activated equilibria is obtained, respectively. Furthermore, Hopf bifurcation occurs at the B-cell-activated equilibrium if the system including the immune response delay. In section 4, numerical simulations are given to further investigate the delays and their effects on the stability of the B-cell-activated equilibrium. Finally, we conclude our work in section 5.
2.
Preliminary results
2.1. Well-posedness
Assuming that system (1.1) satisfies the following initial value:
τ=max{τ1,τ2,τ3,τ4}. Here, ψ2 is a given non-negative constant, ψ1(θ), ψ3(θ), ψ4(θ), ψ5(θ)∈C([−τ,0], R+) with R+=[0,+∞), and ψ=(ψ1,ψ2,ψ3,ψ4,ψ5)∈C×R+×C×C×C.
Theorem 2.1.For system (1.1), there exists a unique non-negative solution with initialvalue (2.1), and the solution is ultimately bounded for all t≥0.
Proof. By the basic theory of the functional differential equations [30], there exists a unique solution satisfying the initial condition (2.1). According to the proof process in Theorem 3.1 of reference [20], we can obtain the nonnegativity of the solution and we omit the proof process here. In the following, we prove the boundedness of the solution.
By the first equation of system (1.1), we calculate that limt→+∞supT(t)≤T0, and see Eq (2.2) for the expression of T0.
which indicates that T(t), L(t), I(t), V(t) and B(t) are all ultimately bounded. This completes the proof.
Suppose that there exists a positive constant M>0 such that T≤T0,L,I,V,B≤M for large t. We will analyze the dynamics of system (1.1) in the following bounded feasible region
Γ={X=(T,L,I,V,B)∈C×R+×C×C×C:T≤T0,L,I,V,B≤M}.
2.2. Reproduction numbers and equilibria
System (1.1) always has one uninfected equilibrium E0(T0,0,0,0,0), where
To study the stability at equilibrium ˉE(ˉT,ˉL,ˉI,ˉV,ˉB), we let Y1(t)=T(t)−ˉT, Y2(t)=L(t)−ˉL, Y3(t)=I(t)−ˉI, Y4(t)=V(t)−ˉV, Y5(t)=B(t)−ˉB, and Y(t)=(Y1(t),Y2(t),Y3(t),Y4(t),Y5(t)). The linearization of system (1.1) at ˉE(ˉT,ˉL,ˉI,ˉV,ˉB) becomes
If R0<1, the above inequality does not hold. Therefore, Eq (3.3) has only negative real part roots. Hence, E0 is locally asymptotically stable if R0<1 for all τi≥0, i=1,2,3,4.
If R0>1, g1(0)=d3d4(α+d2)−kN0ρ(τ1,τ2)(α+d2)=d3d4(α+d2)(1−R0)<0, and limξ→+∞g1(ξ)=+∞. So, Eq (3.3) has at least one positive real root. Therefore, the uninfected equilibrium E0 is unstable for all time delays.
Theorem 3.2.If R0<1, the uninfected equilibrium E0 is globally asymptoticallystable for τi≥0, i=1,2,3,4.
Proof. We employ a particular function
G(x)=x−1−lnx,
and it was first proposed in literature [33]. The function G(x)>0 for x>0, and G(x)=0 when and only when x=1. We define a Lyapunov function W: C×R+×C×C×C→R
If R0<1, then ˙W≤0. Clearly, the singleton E0 is the largest invariant set in {X∈Γ|˙W=0}. By the LaSalle's invariance principle [34], we obtain the global attractivity of E0. Combining with Theorem 3.1, we derive the global asymptotic stability of E0.
3.2. Global asymptotic stability of B-cell-inactivated equilibrium E1
Define a critical condition
R2=qV1d5.
Theorem 3.3.If R2<1<R0, then the B-cell-inactivated equilibrium E1 is locallyasymptotically stable for τi≥0, i=1,2,3,4, and it is unstable if R2>1.
Proof. The characteristic equation of the linearized system (3.1) at the B-cell-inactivated equilibrium E1 is
If τ4=0, then ξ=qV1−d5. Under the condition R2<1, that is qV1−d5<0. If τ4>0, suppose ξ=iω (ω>0), then Eq (3.6) can be written as
{d5=qV1cosξω,ω=−qV1sinξω.
That is to say,
ω2=(qV1)2−d25=(qV1+d5)(qV1−d5).
Under the condition R2<1, there is no positive ω such that the above equation holds. Therefore, Eq (3.6) only has negative real roots. If R2>1, then Eq (3.6) has positive real roots.
Assume ξ=x+iy (x≥0) is a solution of Eq (3.8), and we take the modulus on both sides. Clearly, the modulus of the left hand side of Eq (3.8) is greater than one. Suppose that the modulus of the right hand side of Eq (3.8) is Λ1, use Eq (3.5) and equation
If R∗>1, that is R0>1 (see Eq (2.5)), then Λ1<1, which leads to a contradiction. Therefore, Eq (3.7) has only negative real part roots.
Combined with the above two steps, we conclude that, under the condition R2<1<R0, E1 is locally asymptotically stable for all time delays, and it is unstable if R2>1.
Theorem 3.4.If R2<1<R0, then the B-cell-inactivated equilibrium E1 is globally asymptoticallystable for τi≥0, i=1,2,3,4.
That is, T(t)=T1, L(t)=L1, I(t)=I1, V(t)=V1 and B(t)=0. It is obvious that the singleton E1 is the largest invariant set in {X∈Γ|˙U=0}. By the LaSalle's invariance principle [34], we obtain the global attractivity of E1. Combining with Theorem 3.3, if R2<1<R0, we conclude the global asymptotic stability of E1 for τi≥0, i=1,2,3,4.
3.3. Global asymptotic stability of the B-cell-activated equilibrium E2 if τ4=0
Theorem 3.5.If R1>1, the B-cell-activated equilibrium E2 is locally asymptotically stable for τi≥0, i=1,2,3 and τ4=0.
Proof. The characteristic equation of the linearized system (3.1) at the infected state E2 is
That is, T(t)=T2, L(t)=L2, I(t)=I2, V(t)=V2 and B(t)=B2. Clearly, the singleton E2 is the largest invariant set in {X∈Γ|˙Q=0}. By the LaSalle's invariance principle [34], we obtain the global attractivity of E2. Combining with Theorem 3.5, under the condition R1>1, we derive the global asymptotic stability of E2 for τi≥0, i=1,2,3 and τ4=0.
3.4. Stability of the B-cell-activated equilibrium E2 if τ4>0
For the characteristic Eq (3.9) at the B-cell-activated equilibrium E2, let τ1=τ2=τ3=0, then it yields
From the expressions of m0 in Eq (3.14), we know that we could not determine the sign of m0, and it depends on the size of R1. According to the relationship between root and coefficient, from the expressions of m4 in Eq (3.14), we derive that Eq (3.15) has at most four positive real roots.
Theorem 3.7.Assume R1>1 and τ1=τ2=τ3=0, then the B-cell-activated equilibrium E2 is locally asymptotically stable when τ4∈[0,τ40) and unstable when τ4>τ40. Furthermore, if H′(ω20)≠0, system (1.1) undergoes a Hopf bifurcation to periodic solutions at E2 when τ4=τ40.
Proof. Without loss of generality, suppose that Eq (3.15) has four positive real roots z1,z2, z3 and z4. Then Eq (3.13) has positive roots ωi=√zi(i=1,2,3,4), and thus ±iωi is a pair of purely imaginary roots of Eq (3.11). From Eq (3.12), the corresponding τ4i are
Therefore, a Hopf bifurcation occurs at τ4=τ40 under the condition H′(ω20)≠0. Furthermore, a family of periodic solutions occurs for system (1.1) when τ4>τ40 and τi=0, i=1,2,3.
4.
Numerical simulations
In this section, we perform numerical simulations to show the dynamical behaviour at the B-cell-activated equilibrium E2 of system (1.1), for different values of the cell infection delay τ2 and the immune system delay τ4. We choose parameter values from the modelling literature (see Table 1), and let a=b=0.015 in the following numerical simulations.
In order to study the effect of cell infection delay on the dynamical behaviour of system (1.1), we fix τ1=τ3=1, τ4=0 and choose τ2=0, τ2=2, τ2=6 and τ2=12, respectively. Using Data values in Table 1, we calculate that R0>1 and R1>1 when τ2=0,τ2=2 and τ2=6, while R0<1 when τ2=12. From Figure 1, we can see that, the B-cell activated equilibrium E2 is globally asymptotically stable for τ2 equals 0, 2 and 6, respectively, which is consistent with Theorem 3.6; the uninfected equilibrium E0 is globally asymptotically stable for τ2=12, which is consistent with Theorem 3.2. Moreover, as the cell infection delay τ2 increases, both the number of actively infected T-cells (I) and B-cells (B) decrease, while the number of healthy T-cells (T) and virus (V) remain unchanged. It is also observed that the cell infection delay τ2 can cause the T-cell, virus and B-cell populations to oscillate in the early stage of infection, and prolong the time for system to reach the B-cell-activated equilibrium. In fact, for the immune delay τ4=0, when system (1.1) has only the latency delay τ1 or the virus maturation delay τ3 and the remaining delay values are fixed, we can obtain graphs similar to Figure 1 (not shown here).
Figure 1.
System (1.1) with τ1=τ3=1 and τ4=0, R1>1 when τ2=0, 2, 6, while R0<1 when τ2 = 12. Populations T, I, V and B are shown when τ2=0,2,6,12, respectively.
Next, we investigate the system behaviour at the B-cell-activated equilibrium E2 when the immune delay τ4>0 and τ1=τ2=τ3=0. Choose β=0.0032 and all the other parameter values can be seen in Table 1. By calculating, we obtain that R1=1.2781>1, Eq (3.13) has only one positive real root ω≈0.0925, and τ40≈4.9066. In this situation, Theorem 3.7 is satisfied. Figure 2 shows that the B-cell-activated equilibrium is locally asymptotically stable when the immune time delay τ4=4<τ40, while Figure 3 demonstrates that Hopf bifurcation and period solutions occur at the B-cell-activated equilibrium when τ4=7>τ40.
Now, we examine the dynamics of system (1.1) when all time delays τi>0, i=1,2,3,4. The two delays τ1=1 and τ3=1 are fixed, and all the other parameter values are from Table 1 in the following numerical simulations. Let τ4=6 be fixed, and τ2∈(0,12] varies. With the increase of the cell infection delay τ2, from the left panel of Figure 4, we observe that, (ⅰ) the system firstly undergoes the Hopf bifurcation at the B-cell-activated equilibrium E2, and the amplitude of the periodic solution decreases gradually; (ⅱ) then the system experiences the local asymptotic stability of the B-cell-inactivated equilibrium E1, and finally reaches the local asymptotic stability of the uninfected equilibrium E0. Similar figures can be obtained when τ1 or τ3 varies and the other delays are fixed (not shown here). Moreover, let τ2=2 be fixed, and τ4∈(0,20] varies. With the increase of the immune delay τ4, from the right panel of Figure 4, we find that, the system firstly reaches the local asymptotic stability of the B-cell-activated equilibrium E2, then undergoes the Hopf bifurcation at the B-cell-activated equilibrium. In fact, the periodic solutions still exist when τ4 is greater than 20 (not shown here).
In this paper, we have formulated an HIV infection model including the latently infected cell, the Beddington-DeAngelis infection function, the B-cell immune response and multiple delays. The latency delay, the cell infection delay, the virus maturation delay and the immune delay of B-cell activation are considered in our model. Two basic reproduction numbers and three equilibria are obtained. Theoretically, by constructing the appropriate Lyapunov function, we obtain that both the uninfected and B-cell-inactivated equilibria are globally asymptotically stable for the four time delays. The B-cell-activated equilibrium is globally asymptotically stable for the first three delays, while the immune delay may destabilize the stability of the B-cell-activated equilibrium and lead to Hopf bifurcation.
It is a challenging problem to analyze system (1.1) for the joint effect of four time delays theoretically. So, numerical simulations are performed to investigate the dynamical behaviour at the B-cell-activated equilibrium E2 for τi>0i=1,2,3,4. It was discovered that, the latency delay τ1, the cell infection delay τ2, and the virus maturation delay τ3 can stabilize the B-cell-activated equilibrium, and the bifurcation disappears gradually; while the immune response delay τ4 can lead to its instability, and bifurcation occurs.
As far as we know, HIV infection model considering the latent infection, the Beddington-DeAngelis incidence function and the B-cell immune response is rare, and the theoretical study on the analysis of multiple time delay models is also rare. In this paper, we analyze the global stability of the equilibrium through constructing suitable Lyapunov functions, which is a generalization of the existing models.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank the referees for their valuable suggestions. This work is supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 11801566, No. 11871473, No. 11401589), the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (No. 18CX02049A), and Shandong Provincial Natural Science Foundation.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
A.G Lentoor is a professor of clinical psychology and a recipient of the National Research Foundation Thuthuka Grant, Biological Psychiatry Early-mid career Development Grant and SMU Capacity Development Grant. This work has been submitted as part of a Master in Applied Neuroscience. https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7838-2721
Toglia MP, Schmuller J, Surprenant BG, et al. (2022) Novel Approaches and Cognitive Neuroscience Perspectives on False Memory and Deception. Front Psychol 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.721961
[4]
LaBar KS, Cabeza R (2006) Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 7: 54-64. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1825
[5]
Yu J, Tao Q, Zhang R, et al. (2019) Can fMRI discriminate between deception and false memory? A meta-analytic comparison between deception and false memory studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 104: 43-55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.027
[6]
Bamatraf S, Hussain M, Aboalsamh H, et al. (2016) A System for True and False Memory Prediction Based on 2D and 3D Educational Contents and EEG Brain Signals. Comput Intell Neurosci 2016: 8491046. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/8491046
[7]
Buck S, Sidhu MK (2020) A Guide to Designing a Memory fMRI Paradigm for Pre-surgical Evaluation in Temporal Lobe Epilepsy. Front Neurol 10: 1354. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2019.01354
[8]
Murty VP, Ritchey M, Adcock RA, et al. (2010) fMRI studies of successful emotional memory encoding: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 48: 3459-3469. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.030
[9]
Shaw J (2020) Do False Memories Look Real? Evidence That People Struggle to Identify Rich False Memories of Committing Crime and Other Emotional Events. Front Psychol 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00650
[10]
Jeye BM, Karanian JM, Slotnick SD (2017) The Anterior Prefrontal Cortex and the Hippocampus Are Negatively Correlated during False Memories. Brain Sci 7: 13. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7010013
[11]
Turk KW, Palumbo R, Deason RG, et al. (2020) False memories: The other side of forgetting. J Int Neuropsychol Soc JINS 26: 545-556. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617720000016
[12]
Iidaka T, Harada T, Sadato N (2014) False memory for face in short-term memory and neural activity in human amygdala. Brain Res 1591: 74-85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.10.003
[13]
Kloft L, Otgaar H, Blokland A, et al. (2020) Cannabis increases susceptibility to false memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci 117: 4585-4589. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1920162117
[14]
Metcalfe J, Eich TS (2019) Memory and truth: correcting errors with true feedback versus overwriting correct answers with errors. Cogn Res Princ Implic 4: 4. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-019-0153-8
[15]
Leon CS, Bonilla M, Brusco LI, et al. (2023) Fake news and false memory formation in the psychology debate. IBRO Neurosci Rep 15: 24-30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ibneur.2023.06.002
[16]
St. Jacques PL, Olm C, Schacter DL (2013) Neural mechanisms of reactivation-induced updating that enhance and distort memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci 110: 19671-19678. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1319630110
[17]
Otgaar H, Curci A, Mangiulli I, et al. (2022) A court ruled case on therapy-induced false memories. J Forensic Sci 67: 2122-2129. https://doi.org/10.1111/1556-4029.15073
[18]
Zhu B, Chen C, Shao X, et al. (2019) Multiple interactive memory representations underlie the induction of false memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci 116: 3466-3475. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1817925116
[19]
Straube B (2012) An overview of the neuro-cognitive processes involved in the encoding, consolidation, and retrieval of true and false memories. Behav Brain Funct 8: 35. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-8-35
[20]
Richards BA, Lillicrap TP (2022) The Brain-Computer Metaphor Debate Is Useless: A Matter of Semantics. Front Comput Sci 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomp.2022.810358
[21]
Langille JJ, Brown RE (2018) The Synaptic Theory of Memory: A Historical Survey and Reconciliation of Recent Opposition. Front Syst Neurosci 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2018.00052
[22]
Du X (2022) An Illustration of the False Memory Process Based on The Artificial Neural Networks Model. 2022 2nd International Conference on Bioinformatics and Intelligent Computing . New York, NY, USA: Association for Computing Machinery 484-488. https://doi.org/10.1145/3523286.3524591
Takeuchi T, Duszkiewicz AJ, Morris RGM (2014) The synaptic plasticity and memory hypothesis: encoding, storage and persistence. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 369: 20130288. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0288
[25]
Neves G, Cooke SF, Bliss TVP (2008) Synaptic plasticity, memory and the hippocampus: a neural network approach to causality. Nat Rev Neurosci 9: 65-75. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2303
Zhu B, Chen C, Loftus EF, et al. (2010) Individual differences in false memory from misinformation: Personality characteristics and their interactions with cognitive abilities. Personal Individ Differ 48: 889-894. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.016
[29]
Pardilla-Delgado E, Payne JD (2017) The impact of sleep on true and false memory across long delays. Neurobiol Learn Mem 137: 123-133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.11.016
[30]
Fandakova Y, Sander MC, Grandy TH, et al. (2018) Age differences in false memory: The importance of retrieval monitoring processes and their modulation by memory quality. Psychol Aging 33: 119-133. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000212
[31]
Malloggi S, Conte F, De Rosa O, et al. (2022) False memories formation is increased in individuals with insomnia. J Sleep Res 31: e13527. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13527
[32]
Kersten AW, Earles JL (2017) Feelings of familiarity and false memory for specific associations resulting from mugshot exposure. Mem Cognit 45: 93-104. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-016-0642-7
Singh D, Norman KA, Schapiro AC (2022) A model of autonomous interactions between hippocampus and neocortex driving sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci 119: e2123432119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2123432119
[35]
Takase R, Boasen J, Kuriki S, et al. (2022) Processing time affects sequential memory performance beginning at the level of visual encoding. PLOS ONE 17: e0265719. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0265719
Knott LM, Howe ML, Toffalini E, et al. (2018) The Role of Attention in Immediate Emotional False Memory Enhancement. Emot Wash DC 18: 1063-1077. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000407
[39]
Hellenthal MV, Knott LM, Howe ML, et al. (2019) The effects of arousal and attention on emotional false memory formation. J Mem Lang 107: 54-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2019.03.010
[40]
Sharma PR, Wade KA, Jobson L (2023) A systematic review of the relationship between emotion and susceptibility to misinformation. Memory 31: 1-21. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2022.2120623
[41]
Schacter DL (2012) Adaptive constructive processes and the future of memory. Am Psychol 67: 10.1037/a0029869. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029869
Reyna VF, Corbin JC, Weldon RB, et al. (2016) How Fuzzy-Trace Theory Predicts True and False Memories for Words, Sentences, and Narratives. J Appl Res Mem Cogn 5: 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2015.12.003
Gallo D (2013) Associative Illusions of Memory: False Memory Research in DRM and Related Tasks. Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203782934
[46]
Senese VP, Sergi I, Iachini T (2010) Comparison of activation level between true and false items in the DRM paradigm. Cogn Process 11: 213-217. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-009-0271-1
Abe N, Fujii T, Suzuki M, et al. (2013) Encoding- and retrieval-related brain activity underlying false recognition. Neurosci Res 76: 240-250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2013.05.006
[49]
Zhou N, Zhao C, Yang T, et al. (2018) Attentional bias towards sleep-related stimuli in insomnia disorder: a behavioural and ERP study. J Sleep Res 27: e12652. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12652
[50]
Wagner U, Schlechter P, Echterhoff G (2022) Socially induced false memories in the absence of misinformation. Sci Rep 12: 7725. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11749-w
[51]
Torres EE The Relationship Between Sleep and False Memory (2020) 5.
[52]
Carpenter AC, Schacter DL (2017) Flexible retrieval: When true inferences produce false memories. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 43: 335-349. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000340
[53]
Goodwin KA (2013) Reducing False Memories via Context Reinstatement: The Roles of Encoding and Retrieval Contexts. Am J Psychol 126: 213-225. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.2.0213
[54]
Storbeck J, Clore GL (2011) Affect Influences False Memories at Encoding: Evidence from Recognition Data. Emot Wash DC 11: 981-989. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022754
Okado Y, Stark CEL (2005) Neural activity during encoding predicts false memories created by misinformation. Learn Mem 12: 3-11. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.87605
[57]
Spets DS, Karanian JM, Slotnick SD (2021) False memories activate distinct brain regions in females and males. Neuroimage Rep 1: 100043. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynirp.2021.100043
[58]
Ying GUO, Xianmin G, Dahua W (2021) The cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying false memory: An information processing perspective. Adv Psychol Sci 29: 79. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.00079
[59]
Loftus E (2005) Planting misinformation in the human mind: a 30-year investigation of the malleability of memory. Learn Mem 12: 361-366. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.94705
[60]
Patihis L, Frenda SJ, LePort AKR, et al. (2013) False memories in highly superior autobiographical memory individuals. Proc Natl Acad Sci 110: 20947-20952. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1314373110
[61]
Day AJ, Fenn KM (2020) 0116 Opposing Effects of Sleep on The Misinformation Effect: Sleep Promotes and Prevents Memory Distortion. Sleep 43: A46. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsaa056.114
[62]
Chatburn A, Kohler MJ, Payne JD, et al. (2017) The effects of sleep restriction and sleep deprivation in producing false memories. Neurobiol Learn Mem 137: 107-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.11.017
Tukker JJ, Beed P, Schmitz D, et al. (2020) Up and Down States and Memory Consolidation Across Somatosensory, Entorhinal, and Hippocampal Cortices. Front Syst Neurosci 14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2020.00022
[65]
Cross ZR, Kohler MJ, Schlesewsky M, et al. (2018) Sleep-Dependent Memory Consolidation and Incremental Sentence Comprehension: Computational Dependencies during Language Learning as Revealed by Neuronal Oscillations. Front Hum Neurosci 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00018
[66]
Newbury CR, Monaghan P (2019) When does sleep affect veridical and false memory consolidation? A meta-analysis. Psychon Bull Rev 26: 387-400. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1528-4
[67]
Maraver MJ, Lapa A, Garcia-Marques L, et al. (2022) Can we learn from errors? Retrieval facilitates the correction of false memories for pragmatic inferences. PloS One 17: e0272427. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0272427
[68]
Saraiva M, Garrido MV, Albuquerque PB (2021) Emergence and transmission of misinformation in the context of social interactions. Mem Cognit 49: 1-13. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-020-01081-x
[69]
LePort AKR, Stark SM, McGaugh JL, et al. (2016) Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory: Quality and Quantity of Retention Over Time. Front Psychol 6: 2017. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02017
[70]
Santangelo V, Pedale T, Macrì S, et al. (2020) Enhanced cortical specialization to distinguish older and newer memories in highly superior autobiographical memory. Cortex J Devoted Study Nerv Syst Behav 129: 476-483. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.04.029
[71]
Santangelo V, Cavallina C, Colucci P, et al. (2018) Enhanced brain activity associated with memory access in highly superior autobiographical memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115: 7795-7800. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1802730115
[72]
Santangelo V, Pedale T, Colucci P, et al. (2021) Highly superior autobiographical memory in aging: A single case study. Cortex J Devoted Study Nerv Syst Behav 143: 267-280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2021.05.011
[73]
Dang X, Zhu B, Chen C, et al. (2020) Age differences in false memories for visual scenes and the effect of prior recall. J Pac Rim Psychol 14: e4. https://doi.org/10.1017/prp.2019.26
[74]
Loftus EF, Hoffman HG, Wagenaar WA (1992) The Misinformation Effect: Transformations in Memory Induced by Postevent Information. Development of Long-Term Retention . New York, NY: Springer 159-183. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2868-4_5
Urgolites ZJ, Smith CN, Squire LR (2015) True and false memories, parietal cortex, and confidence judgments. Learn Mem 22: 557-562. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.038349.115
[77]
Zhuang L, Wang J, Xiong B, et al. (2022) Rapid neural reorganization during retrieval practice predicts subsequent long-term retention and false memory. Nat Hum Behav 6: 134-145. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01188-4
[78]
Shao X, Li A, Chen C, et al. (2023) Cross-stage neural pattern similarity in the hippocampus predicts false memory derived from post-event inaccurate information. Nat Commun 14: 2299. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-38046-y
This article has been cited by:
1.
Chinnathambi Rajivganthi, Fathalla A. Rihan,
Global Dynamics of a Stochastic Viral Infection Model with Latently Infected Cells,
2021,
11,
2076-3417,
10484,
10.3390/app112110484
2.
Fei Li, Suxia Zhang, Xiaxia Xu,
Dynamical analysis and optimal control for a delayed viral infection model,
2023,
16,
1793-5245,
10.1142/S1793524522500930
3.
C. Rajivganthi, F. A. Rihan,
Global Dynamics of a Delayed Fractional-Order Viral Infection Model With Latently Infected Cells,
2021,
7,
2297-4687,
10.3389/fams.2021.771662
4.
Saima Rashid, Rehana Ashraf, Qurat-Ul-Ain Asif, Fahd Jarad,
Novel stochastic dynamics of a fractal-fractional immune effector response to viral infection via latently infectious tissues,
2022,
19,
1551-0018,
11563,
10.3934/mbe.2022539
5.
Stanca M. Ciupe, Jessica M. Conway,
Incorporating Intracellular Processes in Virus Dynamics Models,
2024,
12,
2076-2607,
900,
10.3390/microorganisms12050900
6.
A. M. Elaiw, E. A. Almohaimeed, A. D. Hobiny,
Stability of HHV-8 and HIV-1 co-infection model with latent reservoirs and multiple distributed delays,
2024,
9,
2473-6988,
19195,
10.3934/math.2024936
7.
Jinhu Xu, Guokun Huang,
Global Stability and Bifurcation Analysis of a Virus Infection Model with Nonlinear Incidence and Multiple Delays,
2023,
7,
2504-3110,
583,
10.3390/fractalfract7080583
8.
Yuncong Liu, Yan Wang, Daqing Jiang,
Dynamic behaviors of a stochastic virus infection model with Beddington–DeAngelis incidence function, eclipse-stage and Ornstein–Uhlenbeck process,
2024,
369,
00255564,
109154,
10.1016/j.mbs.2024.109154
9.
A. M. Elaiw, E. Dahy, H. Z. Zidan, A. A. Abdellatif,
Stability of an HIV-1 abortive infection model with antibody immunity and delayed inflammatory cytokine production,
2025,
140,
2190-5444,
10.1140/epjp/s13360-025-06475-x