Citation: John Samelis, Athanasia Kakouri. Hurdle factors minimizing growth of Listeria monocytogenes while counteracting in situ antilisterial effects of a novel nisin A-producing Lactococcus lactis subsp. cremoris costarter in thermized cheese milks[J]. AIMS Microbiology, 2018, 4(1): 19-41. doi: 10.3934/microbiol.2018.1.19
[1] | Daniel A. Barone, Ana C. Krieger . The Function of Sleep. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(2): 71-90. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.2.71 |
[2] | Eduardo Mercado III . Relating Cortical Wave Dynamics to Learning and Remembering. AIMS Neuroscience, 2014, 1(3): 185-209. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2014.3.185 |
[3] | Alexis M. Chambers, Jessica D. Payne . The Influence of Sleep on the Consolidation of Positive Emotional Memories: Preliminary Evidence. AIMS Neuroscience, 2014, 1(1): 39-51. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2014.1.39 |
[4] | Samson Z Assefa, Montserrat Diaz-Abad, Emerson M Wickwire, Steven M Scharf . The Functions of Sleep. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(3): 155-171. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.3.155 |
[5] | Marie Vandekerckhove, Yu-lin Wang . Emotion, emotion regulation and sleep: An intimate relationship. AIMS Neuroscience, 2018, 5(1): 1-17. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.1.1 |
[6] | Mark R. Zielinski, James T. McKenna, Robert W. McCarley . Functions and Mechanisms of Sleep. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(1): 67-104. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.1.67 |
[7] | Kimberly A Cote, Catherine E Milner, Tamara A Speth . Altered Sleep Mechanisms following Traumatic Brain Injury and Relation to Waking Function. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(4): 203-228. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.4.203 |
[8] | Carolyn E. Jones, Miranda M. Lim . Phasic Sleep Events Shape Cognitive Function after Traumatic Brain Injury: Implications for the Study of Sleep in Neurodevelopmental Disorders. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(2): 232-236. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.2.232 |
[9] | Ritwik Das, Artur Luczak . Epileptic seizures and link to memory processes. AIMS Neuroscience, 2022, 9(1): 114-127. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2022007 |
[10] | Shun Sakuma, Yuko Mizuno-Matsumoto, Yoshi Nishitani, Shinichi Tamura . Learning Times Required to Identify the Stimulated Position and Shortening of Propagation Path by Hebb’s Rule in Neural Network. AIMS Neuroscience, 2017, 4(4): 238-253. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2017.4.238 |
Originally likened to a wax tablet by Plato and Aristotle [1], memory has been the muse of philosophers and scientists alike. The discovery of the synapse separating individual neurons by early neuroscientists at the turn of the 20th century, and the subsequent proposal that increased activity at these synapses strengthens the connection between neurons [2] were pivotal ideas for expanding our understanding of memory beyond a philosophical concept or observed behavior to a complex process located in the brain. Today, we know that synaptic plasticity is important for memory formation, but there is still much to learn about how memory is formed at the neural level. As Cadonic and Albensi [3] point out, neural oscillations and receptors implicated in brain plasticity may hold the key to unlocking the mystery of how we form memories. Another essential player in this relationship, increasingly receiving support for its role in various memory processes, is sleep.
The ability to form new memories relies on synaptic plasticity, whereby high-frequency activation of a neural pathway increases the strength of later transmission across the affected synapses, a process termed long term potentiation (LTP) [4,5]. Amino-acid receptors have been implicated in the process of LTP, including the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor and the alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptor. During LTP, the NMDA receptor is first activated when both the Mg2+ blocking its channel is removed by depolarization, and co-agonists glutamate and glycine are bound to its receptors [5,6]. This allows Ca2+ to flow into the cell, which initiates a cascade of events, including the activation of synaptic proteins and the phosphorylation and transfer of AMPA receptors into the synapse, culminating in the strengthening of excitatory postsynaptic currents [7]. LTP consists of two phases: the initial phase may last for hours, while the longer maintenance phase lasts for weeks [8]. Many studies connect LTP to the formation of new memories. For instance, an increase in the phosphorylation and number of AMPA receptors at the synapse has been found for LTP established via high-frequency stimulation in vitro, as well as during inhibitory avoidance learning in rats [9]. Blocking LTP has also been shown to block new learning [10].
Additionally, glutamatergic receptors like the NMDA receptor are important for the generation of neural oscillations, which have also been connected to memory formation. The rhythmic activity produced by the brain can be measured by electrophysiological measures such as the electroencephalogram (EEG), which has been used to delineate several frequency bands implicated in memory processes, including theta (4-7 Hz), alpha (7-14 Hz), beta (15-30 Hz), and gamma (30-100 Hz) frequencies [11,12]. Previous studies have found that NMDA antagonists block NMDA function and dramatically reduce electrically stimulated oscillations in the 4-9 Hz range [13]. Further, removal of the NMDA receptor in genetically altered mice decreases the power of theta and increases the power of gamma oscillations measured in the hippocampus compared to controls. This alteration negatively impacts spatial working-memory of these mice during a maze-learning task [14], which is in line with the proposal that such oscillations underlie varying types of memory formation and may represent transfer of information between brain regions [15]. In humans, changes in alpha, beta, gamma, and theta power are associated with memory for previously studied words [16], with encoding and retrieval strategies playing a role in the direction of such relationships [12].
While the review by Cadonic and Albensi [3] nicely outlines this relationship between glutamatergic receptors and oscillatory activity, pointing toward an important and fruitful area for future research on memory formation, further information may be provided by including sleep in this discussion, especially for the maintenance of already initiated LTP. Sleep is characterized by the alternation of dynamic stages, including non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREM), composed of stages 1, 2, and 3/4 or slow wave sleep (SWS), and rapid eye movement sleep (REM), each of which can be distinguished by unique physiological, chemical, and electrical properties [17,18]. Most important for the discussion here, NREM sleep, and particularly SWS, is characterized by slow oscillatory EEG patterns in the 0.5-4 Hz range, representing the alternation between up states (i.e., neural activity) and down states (i.e., neural silence), and punctuated by high-frequency bursts known as sleep spindles (12-15 Hz) in the cortex. These spindles are temporally correlated with sharp-wave ripples (150-250 Hz) in the hippocampus [19]. Conversely, REM sleep is characterized by theta EEG activity, which is synchronized with gamma oscillations in the hippocampus [18,20].
One of the most substantiated functions of sleep is its role in brain plasticity supporting both declarative and non-declarative memory processing [19]. The purported mechanism by which sleep has such a ubiquitous effect on memory is the reactivation and consolidation of recently potentiated synapses [21,22]. The Synaptic Re-entry Reinforcement hypothesis [21,22,23] proposes that the longevity of brain plasticity requires a reactivation of NMDA receptors following the initial learning event. Indeed, without proper synaptic stimulation and protein synthesis, LTP does not last beyond the several hours of its initial phase [24,25]. Thus, following the initiation of LTP during a learning event, potentiated synapses require reactivation to maintain synaptic plasticity. Sleep offers an optimum context for such reactivation to occur, given that activation of glutamate receptors and generation of neural oscillations are hallmarks of sleep processes [19].
Evidence for the reactivation of glutamatergic receptor mechanisms at potentiated synapses during sleep is abundant. Following monocular deprivation learning, whereby one eye is occluded to prevent input of visual information, neural activity becomes biased toward the region of visual cortex corresponding to the non-deprived eye, and this is further enhanced by a period of sleep. This effect is promoted by reactivation of potentiated pathways during sleep, as blocking NMDA receptors or the synthesis of related downstream proteins during a post-learning sleep period negates the consolidation of this plasticity. Further, phosphorylation of AMPA receptors is found only in animals allowed to sleep after monocular deprivation [22]. In humans, the blockade of NMDA or AMPA receptors during an eight-hour period of nocturnal, post-learning sleep likewise abolishes typical sleep-dependent improvements in visual discrimination learning [21]. Moreover, sleep deprivation, which is known to impair learning, has been shown to negatively alter glutamate receptor activation and protein cascades necessary for LTP [19,26]. This work highlights the importance of sleep for glutamatergic receptor signaling that maintains plasticity initiated during a waking period, without which learning would be impeded.
As others have pointed out, the oscillatory activity that occurs during sleep is similar to that supporting induction of LTP [19]. This may make sleep-related oscillations ideal for the reactivation of potentiated synapses. NREM sleep in particular has been the focus of many studies on this matter [27,28,29]. Indeed, some studies have found that under anesthesia or during waking, synapses previously strengthened by LTP are more likely to generate slow oscillatory up states and spindle activity [30,31]. Further, there is a reciprocal relationship between spindle activity and glutamatergic receptors, such that NMDA inputs mediate spindle activity [13], and stimulation of spindle activity in the cortex influences NMDA receptors and Ca2+, leading to the induction of LTP [29]. Consistently, neural electrical stimulation during in vitro imitations of the electrophysiological states that characterize SWS, including the combination of up states and down states, facilitates LTP and is blocked by NMDA or AMPA antagonists [28]. In vivo applications of slow oscillatory activity (0.75 Hz) during NREM sleep following the learning of word-pairs not only increases SWS, slow oscillatory power, and slow spindle power, but also results in better memory for the learned word-pairs compared to a control condition. Such effects are specific to slow oscillations, as theta frequency stimulation does not produce these results [27]. This evidence suggests a close relationship between the electrical activity of NREM sleep and activation of glutamate receptors that contribute to learning, providing evidence for the role of NREM sleep in reactivation and maintenance of LTP.
REM sleep is also implicated in LTP reprocessing. During REM sleep, but not NREM sleep, proteins necessary for the long-term maintenance of already established LTP, such as cAMP (cyclic adenosine monophosphate) and CREB (cAMP-response element binding protein), are activated at higher levels compared to a period of wakefulness, a process not found in mice genetically altered to lack memory consolidation abilities [19,32]. Antagonists acting on specific subunits of NMDA receptors also increase gamma activity during REM sleep [33]. This finding is interesting given that theta and gamma oscillations, which are synchronized within regions of the hippocampus during REM sleep with periodic bursts of synchrony across areas, are proposed to play a role in memory processing and transfer during sleep [20]. Theta and gamma oscillations are also temporally associated between hemispheres during REM, which may be particularly important for neural processes given that recovery sleep following REM deprivation produces a rebound and intensification of gamma synchronization between the left and right frontopolar and dorsolateral regions of the frontal lobe [34]. Importantly, stimulation in the gamma range across the corpus callosum initiates a short-lived potentiation that is blocked with NMDA antagonists, implicating this oscillatory connection between hemispheres during REM sleep as another mechanism for the sleep-associated maintenance of plasticity in neural circuits [34,35].
As some of the most interesting evidence for neuronal replay during sleep, hippocampal place cells that were activated during spatial learning during a waking period are sequentially reactivated on a compressed timescale during subsequent NREM and on a timescale comparable to learning during subsequent REM [36,37]. Important to the discussion here, replay is associated with sharp wave ripples in the hippocampus during NREM sleep, and theta activity during REM sleep. Given that these electrical markers of sleep have been correlated with memory improvements [38,39], such replay is proposed to reflect a mechanism of memory consolidation through an exchange in communication between the hippocampus and cortex [18,40].
It is important to note that another hypothesis regarding sleep and neural plasticity, the Synaptic Homeostasis Hypothesis [41,42], proposes that sleep generally downscales the potentiation of synapses that has accumulated over a period wakefulness, thus maintaining neural stability [42]. This hypothesis puts particular emphasis on SWS and slow oscillations, which are seen to increase after waking, and decrease after sleep [42]. While seemingly at odds with the studies discussed thus far, which show that sleep processes help maintain LTP, others have suggested that synaptic downscaling and maintenance may both occur during SWS, depending on how the plasticity relates to different types of learning [22], and how it may develop in different neural pathways that differ in their requirements for plasticity [28].
As Cadonic and Albensi [3] point out, abnormal NMDA receptor signaling also likely plays a role in pathology, such that hypo- and hyperactivation has been associated with Schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease, respectively. Not surprisingly, evidence additionally indicates oscillatory activity, and sleep, in the etiology of such diseases. Wamsley and colleagues [43] have demonstrated that Schizophrenia patients display a reduction in the number and density of sleep spindles during a period of sleep following motor task learning, which is associated with a reduction in motor performance following sleep compared to controls. High-frequency (> 20 Hz) oscillatory power is also increased during sleep in Schizophrenic and depressed patients compared to healthy individuals [44]. While these studies do not clarify exact causal relationships, they reaffirm the role of sleep in brain plasticity and cognitive functioning, and may provide clues to developing treatments.
Ample research points toward sleep as a crucial contributor to neural plasticity and learning. The dynamic neurophysiology of sleep provides an optimum milieu for the reactivation of recently potentiated synapses and transfer of information to cortical stores, with alterations of this environment linked to pathology. Given the foregoing discussion, Cadonic and Albensi [3] are accurate to champion the further investigation of plasticity-related receptors and oscillations in future memory research, but the addition of sleep to this endeavor will greatly expand the progress we can make to ultimately understanding how we remember and learn.
All the authors declare to have no conflict of interest.
[1] |
Lianou A, Sofos JN (2007) A review on the incidence and transmission of Listeria monocytogenes in ready-to-eat products in retail and food service environments. J Food Protect 70: 2172–2198. doi: 10.4315/0362-028X-70.9.2172
![]() |
[2] |
Kousta M, Mataragas M, Skandamis P, et al. (2010) Prevalence and sources of cheese contamination with pathogens at farm and processing levels. Food Control 21: 805–815. doi: 10.1016/j.foodcont.2009.11.015
![]() |
[3] |
Verraes C, Vlaemynck G, Van Weyenberg S, et al. (2015) A review of the microbiological hazards of dairy products made from raw milk. Int Dairy J 50: 32–44. doi: 10.1016/j.idairyj.2015.05.011
![]() |
[4] | Silk BJ, Mahon BE, Griffin PM, et al. (2013) Vital signs: Listeria illnesses, deaths, and outbreaks-United States, 2009-2011. MMWR-Morbid Mortal W 62: 448–452. |
[5] |
Gould LH, Mungai E, Behravesh CB (2014) Outbreaks attributed to cheese: Differences between outbreaks caused by unpasterurized and pasteurized dairy products, United States, 1998-2011. Foodborne Pathog Dis 11: 545–551. doi: 10.1089/fpd.2013.1650
![]() |
[6] |
Raheem D (2016) Outbreaks of listeriosis associated with deli meats and cheese: an overview. AIMS Microbiol 2: 230–250. doi: 10.3934/microbiol.2016.3.230
![]() |
[7] | Angelidis AS, Govaris A (2012) The behavior of Listeria monocytogenes during the manufacture and storage of Greek Protected Designation of Origin (PDO) cheeses, In: Romano A, Giordano CF, Editors, Listeria Infections: Epidemiology, Pathogenesis and Treatment, Nova Science Publishers, 1–34. |
[8] | Álvarez-Ordóñez A, Leong D, Hickey B, et al. (2015) The challenge of challenge testing to monitor Listeria monocytogenes growth on ready-to-eat foods in Europe by following the European Commission (2014) Technical Guidance document. Food Res Int 75: 233–243. |
[9] | European Commission (2005) Commission Regulation (EC) No 2073/2005 of 15 November 2005 on microbiological criteria for foodstuffs. Official J Eur Union L338: 1–26. |
[10] | European Commission (2007) Commission Regulation (EC) No 1441/2007 of 5 December 2007 amending Regulation (EC) No. 2073/2005 on microbiological criteria for foodstuffs. Official J Eur Union L322: 12–29. |
[11] |
Giannou E, Kakouri A, Matijašic BB, et al. (2009) Fate of Listeria monocytogenes on fully ripened Greek Graviera cheese stored at 4, 12, or 25 °C in air or vacuum packages: in situ PCR detection of a cocktail of bacteriocins potentially contributing to pathogen inhibition. J Food Protect 72: 531–538. doi: 10.4315/0362-028X-72.3.531
![]() |
[12] |
Samelis J, Giannou E, Lianou A (2009) Assuring growth inhibition of listerial contamination during processing and storage of traditional Greek Graviera cheese: compliance with the new European Union regulatory criteria for Listeria monocytogenes. J Food Protect 72: 2264–2271. doi: 10.4315/0362-028X-72.11.2264
![]() |
[13] |
Chatelard-Chauvin C, Pelissier F, Hulin S, et al. (2015) Behaviour of Listeria monocytogenes in raw milk Cantal type cheeses during cheese making, ripening and storage in different packaging conditions. Food Control 54: 53–65. doi: 10.1016/j.foodcont.2015.01.007
![]() |
[14] |
Wemmenhove E, Beumer RR, Van Hooijdonk ACM, et al. (2014) The fate of Listeria monocytogenes in brine and on Gouda cheese following artificial contamination during brining. Int Dairy J 39: 253–258. doi: 10.1016/j.idairyj.2014.06.002
![]() |
[15] |
Kapetanakou AE, Gkerekou MA, Vitzilaiou ES, et al. (2017) Assessing the capacity of growth, survival, and acid adaptive response of Listeria monocytogenes during storage of various cheeses and subsequent simulated gastric digestion. Int J Food Microbiol 246: 50–63. doi: 10.1016/j.ijfoodmicro.2017.01.015
![]() |
[16] |
Shrestha S, Grieder JA, McMahon DJ, et al. (2011) Survival of Listeria monocytogenes introduced as a post-aging contaminant during storage of low-salt Cheddar cheese at 4, 10, and 21 °C. J Dairy Sci 94: 4329–4335. doi: 10.3168/jds.2011-4219
![]() |
[17] |
Dalmasso M, Jordan K (2014) Absence of growth of Listeria monocytogenes in naturally contaminated Cheddar cheese. J Dairy Res 81: 46–53. doi: 10.1017/S0022029913000678
![]() |
[18] |
Adrião A, Vieira M, Fernandes I, et al. (2008) Marked intra-strain variation in response of Listeria monocytogenes dairy isolates to acid or salt stress and the effect of acid or salt adaptation on adherence to abiotic surfaces. Int J Food Microbiol 123: 142–150. doi: 10.1016/j.ijfoodmicro.2007.12.016
![]() |
[19] |
Samelis J, Ikeda JS, Sofos JN (2003) Evaluation of the pH-dependent, stationary-phase acid tolerance in Listeria monocytogenes and Salmonella Typhimurium DT104 induced by culturing in media with 1% glucose: a comparative study with Escherichia coli O157:H7. J Appl Microbiol 95: 563–575. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2672.2003.02013.x
![]() |
[20] |
Samelis J, Kakouri A, Pappa EC, et al. (2010) Microbial stability and safety of traditional Greek Graviera cheese: characterization of the lactic acid bacterial flora and culture-independent detection of bacteriocin genes in the ripened cheeses and their microbial consortia. J Food Protect 73: 1294–1303. doi: 10.4315/0362-028X-73.7.1294
![]() |
[21] |
Wemmenhove E, Van Valenberg HJF, Zwietering MH, et al. (2016) Minimal inhibitory concentrations of undissociated lactic, acetic, citric and propionic acid for Listeria monocytogenes under conditions relevant to cheese. Food Microbiol 58: 63–67. doi: 10.1016/j.fm.2016.03.012
![]() |
[22] |
Angelidis AS, Boutsiouki P, Papageorgiou DK (2010) Loss of viability of Listeria monocytogenes in contaminated processed cheese during storage at 4, 12 and 22 °C. Food Microbiol 27: 809–818. doi: 10.1016/j.fm.2010.04.017
![]() |
[23] |
Wemmenhove E, Stampelou I, van Hooijdonk ACM, et al. (2013) Fate of Listeria monocytogenes in Gouda microcheese: No growth, and substantial inactivation after extended ripening times. Int Dairy J 32: 192–198. doi: 10.1016/j.idairyj.2013.05.004
![]() |
[24] |
Leroy F, De Vuyst L (2004) Lactic acid bacteria as functional starter cultures for the food fermentation industry. Trends Food Sci Tech 15: 67–78. doi: 10.1016/j.tifs.2003.09.004
![]() |
[25] |
Gálvez A, Lopez RL, Abriouel H, et al. (2008) Application of bacteriocins in the control of foodborne pathogenic and spoilage bacteria. Crit Rev Biotechnol 28: 125–152. doi: 10.1080/07388550802107202
![]() |
[26] |
Alvarez-Sieiro P, Montalban-Lopez M, Mu DD, et al. (2016) Bacteriocins of lactic acid bacteria: extending the family. Appl Microbiol Biotechnol 100: 2939–2951. doi: 10.1007/s00253-016-7343-9
![]() |
[27] |
Rodriguez E, Arques JL, Gaya P, et al. (2001) Control of Listeria monocytogenes by bacteriocins and monitoring of bacteriocin-producing lactic acid bacteria by colony hybridization in semi-hard raw milk cheese. J Dairy Res 68: 131–137. doi: 10.1017/S0022029900004660
![]() |
[28] |
Alegria A, Delgado S, Roces C, et al. (2010) Bacteriocins produced by wild Lactococcus lactis strains isolated from traditional, starter-free cheeses made of raw milk. Int J Food Microbiol 143: 61–66. doi: 10.1016/j.ijfoodmicro.2010.07.029
![]() |
[29] |
Dal Bello B, Cocolin L, Zeppa G, et al. (2012) Technological characterization of bacteriocin producing Lactococcus lactis strains employed to control Listeria monocytogenes in Cottage cheese. Int J Food Microbiol 153: 58–65. doi: 10.1016/j.ijfoodmicro.2011.10.016
![]() |
[30] |
Pisano MB, Fadda ME, Melis R, et al. (2015) Molecular identification of bacteriocins produced by Lactococcus lactis dairy strains and their technological and genotypic characterization. Food Control 51: 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.foodcont.2014.11.005
![]() |
[31] | Bouksaim M, Lacroix C, Audet P, et al. (2000) Effects of mixed starter composition on nisin Z production by Lactococcus lactis subsp. lactis biovar. diacetylactis UL 719 during production and ripening of Gouda cheese. Int J Food Microbiol 59: 141–156. |
[32] |
O'Sullivan L, Ryan MP, Ross RP, et al. (2003) Generation of food-grade lactococcal starters which produce the lantibiotics lacticin 3147 and lacticin 481. Appl Environ Microbiol 69: 3681–3685. doi: 10.1128/AEM.69.6.3681-3685.2003
![]() |
[33] | Mills S, Griffin C, O'Connor PM, et al. (2017) A multibacteriocin cheese starter system, comprising nisin and lacticin 3147 in Lactococcus lactis, in combination with plantaricin from Lactobacillus plantarum. Appl Environ Microbiol 83: e00799-17. |
[34] |
Chollet E, Sebti I, Martial-Gros A, et al. (2008) Nisin preliminary study as a potential preservative for sliced ripened cheese: NaCl, fat and enzymes influence on nisin concentration and its antimicrobial activity. Food Control 19: 982–989. doi: 10.1016/j.foodcont.2007.10.005
![]() |
[35] | Benech RO, Kheadr EE, Lacroix C, et al. (2002)Antibacterial activities of nisin Z encapsulated in liposomes or produced in situ by mixed culture during Cheddar cheese ripening. Appl Environ Microbiol 68: 5607–5619. |
[36] | Benkerroum N, Sandine WE (1998) Inhibitory action of nisin against Listeria monocytogenes. J Dairy Sci 71: 3237–3245. |
[37] |
Ferreira MASS, Lund BM (1996) The effect of nisin on Listeria monocytogenes in culture medium and long-life cottage cheese. Lett Appl Microbiol 22: 433–438. doi: 10.1111/j.1472-765X.1996.tb01197.x
![]() |
[38] | Ryser ET (1999) Incidence and behavior of Listeria monocytogenes in cheese and other fermented dairy products, In: Ryser ET, Marth EH, Editors., Listeria, Listeriosis and Food Safety, New York: Marcel Dekker, 411–503. |
[39] |
Collins B, Cotter PD, Hill C, et al. (2011) The impact of nisin on sensitive and resistant mutants of Listeria monocytogenes in cottage cheese. J Appl Microbiol 110: 1509–1514. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2672.2011.05005.x
![]() |
[40] |
Al-Holy MA, Al-Nabulsi A, Osaili TM, et al. (2012) Inactivation of Listeria innocua in brined white cheese by a combination of nisin and heat. Food Control 23: 48–53. doi: 10.1016/j.foodcont.2011.06.009
![]() |
[41] |
Aly S, Floury J, Piot M, et al. (2012) The efficacy of nisin can drastically vary when produced in situ in model cheeses. Food Microbiol 32: 185–190. doi: 10.1016/j.fm.2012.06.001
![]() |
[42] | Dal Bello B, Zeppa G, Bianchi DM, et al. (2013) Effect of nisin-producing Lactococcus lactis starter cultures on the inhibition of two pathogens in ripened cheeses. Int J Dairy Technol 66: 468–477. |
[43] |
Sallami L, Kheadr EE, Fliss I, et al. (2004) Impact of autolytic, proteolytic, and nisin-producing adjunct cultures on biochemical and textural properties of cheddar cheese. J Dairy Sci 87: 1585–1594. doi: 10.3168/jds.S0022-0302(04)73312-3
![]() |
[44] |
Samelis J, Lianou A, Kakouri A, et al. (2009) Changes in the microbial composition of raw milk induced by thermization treatments applied prior to traditional Greek hard cheese processing. J Food Protect 72: 783–790. doi: 10.4315/0362-028X-72.4.783
![]() |
[45] | Parapouli M, Delbés-Paus C, Kakouri A, et al. (2013) Characterization of a wild, novel nisin A-producing Lactococcus strain with an L. lactis subsp. cremoris genotype and an L. lactis subsp. lactis phenotype isolated from Greek raw milk. Appl Environ Microbiol 79: 3476–3484. |
[46] | Samelis J, Giannou E, Pappa EC, et al. (2017) Behavior of artificial listerial contamination in model Greek Graviera cheeses manufactured with the indigenous nisin A-producing strain Lactococcus lactis subsp. cremoris M104 as costarter culture. J Food Safety 37: e12326. |
[47] | Trmčić A, Monnet C, Rogelj I, et al. (2010) Expression of nisin genes in cheese-A quantitative real-time polymerase chain reaction approach. J Dairy Sci 94: 77–85. |
[48] | Lianou A, Samelis J (2014) Addition to thermized milk of Lactococcus lactis subsp. cremoris M104, a wild, novel nisin A-producing strain, replaces the natural antilisterial activity of the autochthonous raw milk microbiota reduced by thermization. J Food Protect 77: 1289–1297. |
[49] | Lianou A, Kakouri A, Pappa EC, et al. (2017) Growth interactions and antilisterial effects of the bacteriocinogenic Lactococcus lactis subsp. cremoris M104 and Enterococcus faecium KE82 strains in thermized milk in the presence or absence of a commercial starter culture. Food Microbiol 64: 145–154. |
[50] | Noutsopoulos D, Kakouri A, Kartezini E, et al. (2017) Growth, nisA gene expression and in situ nisin A activity of novel Lactococcus lactis subsp. cremoris costarter culture in commercial hard cheese production. J Food Protect 80: 2137–2146. |
[51] |
Vandera E, Lianou A, Kakouri A, et al. (2017) Enhanced control of Listeria monocytogenes by Enterococcus faecium KE82, a multiple enterocin-producing strain, in different milk environments. J Food Protect 80: 74–85. doi: 10.4315/0362-028X.JFP-16-082
![]() |
[52] | Schaffner E, Muhlemann M, Spahr U, et al. (2003) Quantification of the probability of milk contamination by Listeria monocytogenes during manufacture of hard cheese. Rev Epidemiol Sante 51:493–503. |
[53] |
Rogga KJ, Samelis J, Kakouri A, et al. (2005) Survival of Listeria monocytogenes in Galotyri, a traditional Greek soft acid-curd cheese, stored aerobically at 4 and 12 °C. Int Dairy J 15: 59–67. doi: 10.1016/j.idairyj.2004.05.002
![]() |
[54] |
Lortal S, Chapot-Chartier MP (2005) Role, mechanisms and control of lactic acid bacteria lysis in cheese. Int Dairy J 15: 857–871. doi: 10.1016/j.idairyj.2004.08.024
![]() |
[55] |
Masoud W, Vogensen FK, Lillevang S, et al. (2012) The fate of indigenous microbiota, starter cultures, Escherichia coli, Listeria innocua and Staphylococcus aureus in Danish raw milk and cheeses determined by pyrosequencing and quantitative real time (qRT)-PCR. Int J Food Microbiol 153: 192–202. doi: 10.1016/j.ijfoodmicro.2011.11.014
![]() |
[56] |
Wemmenhove E, van Valenberg HJF, van Hooijdonk, et al. (2018) Factors that inhibit growth of Listeria monocytogenes in nature-ripened Gouda cheese: A major role for undissociated lactic acid. Food Control 84: 413–418. doi: 10.1016/j.foodcont.2017.08.028
![]() |
[57] | Samelis J, Lianou A, Pappa EC, et al. (2014) Behavior of Staphylococcus aureus in culture broth, in raw and thermized milk, and during processing and storage of traditional Greek Graviera cheese in the presence or absence of Lactococcus lactis subsp. cremoris M104, a wild, novel nisin A-producing raw milk isolate. J Food Protect 77: 1703–1714. |
[58] |
Lekkas C, Kakouri A, Paleologos E, et al. (2006) Survival of Escherichia coli O157:H7 in Galotyri cheese stored at 4 and 12 °C. Food Microbiol 23: 268–276. doi: 10.1016/j.fm.2005.03.008
![]() |
[59] |
Callon C, Saubusse M, Didienne R, et al. (2011) Simplification of a complex microbial antilisterial consortium to evaluate the contribution of its flora in uncooked pressed cheese. Int J Food Microbiol 145: 379–389. doi: 10.1016/j.ijfoodmicro.2010.12.019
![]() |
[60] |
Peláez C, Requena T (2005) Exploiting the potential of bacteria in the cheese ecosystem. Int Dairy J 15: 831–844. doi: 10.1016/j.idairyj.2004.12.001
![]() |
[61] |
Montel MC, Buchin S, Mallet A, et al. (2014) Traditional cheeses: Rich and diverse microbiota with associated benefits. Int J Food Microbiol 177: 136–154. doi: 10.1016/j.ijfoodmicro.2014.02.019
![]() |
[62] |
Schvartzman MS, Belessi C, Butler F, et al. (2011) Effect of pH and water activity on the growth limits of Listeria monocytogenes in a cheese matrix at two contamination levels. J Food Protect 74: 1805–1813. doi: 10.4315/0362-028X.JFP-11-102
![]() |