
Citation: Yoshi Nishitani, Chie Hosokawa, Yuko Mizuno-Matsumoto, Tomomitsu Miyoshi, Shinichi Tamura. Learning process for identifying different types of communication via repetitive stimulation: feasibility study in a cultured neuronal network[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2019, 6(4): 240-249. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2019.4.240
[1] | Yoshi Nishitani, Chie Hosokawa, Yuko Mizuno-Matsumoto, Tomomitsu Miyoshi, Shinichi Tamura . Effect of correlating adjacent neurons for identifying communications: Feasibility experiment in a cultured neuronal network. AIMS Neuroscience, 2018, 5(1): 18-31. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2018.1.18 |
[2] | Yoshi Nishitani, Chie Hosokawa, Yuko Mizuno-Matsumoto, Tomomitsu Miyoshi, Shinichi Tamura . Classification of Spike Wave Propagations in a Cultured Neuronal Network: Investigating a Brain Communication Mechanism. AIMS Neuroscience, 2017, 4(1): 1-13. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2017.1.1 |
[3] | Shinichi Tamura, Yoshi Nishitani, Chie Hosokawa . Feasibility of Multiplex Communication in a 2D Mesh Asynchronous Neural Network with Fluctuations. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(4): 385-397. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.4.385 |
[4] | Shun Sakuma, Yuko Mizuno-Matsumoto, Yoshi Nishitani, Shinichi Tamura . Learning Times Required to Identify the Stimulated Position and Shortening of Propagation Path by Hebb’s Rule in Neural Network. AIMS Neuroscience, 2017, 4(4): 238-253. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2017.4.238 |
[5] | Shun Sakuma, Yuko Mizuno-Matsumoto, Yoshi Nishitani, Shinichi Tamura . Simulation of Spike Wave Propagation and Two-to-one Communication with Dynamic Time Warping. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(4): 474-486. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.4.474 |
[6] | Anastasios A. Mirisis, Anamaria Alexandrescu, Thomas J. Carew, Ashley M. Kopec . The Contribution of Spatial and Temporal Molecular Networks in the Induction of Long-term Memory and Its Underlying Synaptic Plasticity. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(3): 356-384. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.3.356 |
[7] | T. Virmani, F. J. Urbano, V. Bisagno, E. Garcia-Rill . The pedunculopontine nucleus: From posture and locomotion to neuroepigenetics. AIMS Neuroscience, 2019, 6(4): 219-230. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2019.4.219 |
[8] | Dirk Roosterman, Graeme S. Cottrell . Astrocytes and neurons communicate via a monocarboxylic acid shuttle. AIMS Neuroscience, 2020, 7(2): 94-106. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2020007 |
[9] | Eduardo Mercado III . Learning-Related Synaptic Reconfiguration in Hippocampal Networks: Memory Storage or Waveguide Tuning?. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(1): 28-34. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.1.28 |
[10] | Gigi Tevzadze, Tamar Barbakadze, Elisabed Kvergelidze, Elene Zhuravliova, Lali Shanshiashvili, David Mikeladze . Gut neurotoxin p-cresol induces brain-derived neurotrophic factor secretion and increases the expression of neurofilament subunits in PC-12 cells. AIMS Neuroscience, 2022, 9(1): 12-23. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2022002 |
It is well known that various types of information can be learned and memorized. These processes are based on the functions of large neuronal networks that are assembled through spike propagation (action potentials) via synapses [1]–[5] in the brain. These functions are very complex and ambiguous. In such conditions, how can multiple types of information be communicated and stored? How do neuronal networks function in the learning process? These are very important, yet difficult, topics for brain function. Many researchers have attempted to answer these questions. Most previous studies were based on spike-coding metrics [6], spatiotemporal coding models [7]–[13], and synchronous action models [14]–[18]. Regarding data communication in the brain, we reported that spikes propagating from stimulated neurons are received by afferent neurons as random-like sequences in simulated and natural asynchronous neuronal networks [19]–[25]. This phenomenon is similar to radio wave propagation in artificial data communication systems; hence, this phenomenon was termed “spike wave propagation.” In those studies, we showed that stimulated neurons were able to identify various spatiotemporal patterns of spike wave propagation in specific areas (receiving area) of the neuronal network. These results indicated that different types of communication can be identified. From a basic brain function point of view, we consider that the identification of types of communication is related to the identification of different objects using vision by seeing, of different sounds using hearing, etc. [26]–[27]. In contrast, from a learning function point of view, we can expect that the quality of the identification of the different stimulation areas is improved by repeating the stimulation corresponding to the learning process, as they can be identified more accurately via repeated training. However, this has not been confirmed because our previous experiments [24]–[25] used single stimulation exclusively.
Here, we investigated this learning process using repetitive stimulation in cultured neuronal networks as a feasibility study of learning function. This work is an application of the results of our recent study [24]–[25].
Cell culture, stimulated spike recording, and coding spike trains were as described previously [24]–[25]. Hippocampal neurons were dissected from Wistar rats on embryonic day 18. The procedure conformed to the protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology. Cells were cultured individually on an array of 64 planar microelectrodes (8 × 8 configuration). Each electrode had an individual number, from channel 1 (termed ch1 henceforth) to ch64, and corresponded to one neuron as shown in Figure S1 in Supplementary Information. In this study, we prepared 11 cultures, which were termed culture 01–11.
The stimulation method used here was as follows.
(1) Stimulation was repeated 8–12 times at a particular stimulation interval (SI) at one electrode (corresponding to one neuron), as shown in Figure 1.
(2) The procedure described in (1) was performed at an additional two electrodes.
(3) Procedures (1) and (2) were repeated 5–10 times for the back-propagation neural network (BPNN) analysis (see Section 2.3).
In this study, we set SI to 0.5 and 1.5 s and compared the results obtained. The stimulation channels used (termed Stch henceforth) were ch11, 36, and 54.
As described in [25], the time sequence data used for identifying a stimulated neuron (channel) were coded as stimulated spike trains at specific neurons (current channels) and their adjacent (to the left and right) neurons (see Figures 2 and 3(b) in [25]).
We analyzed the temporal patterns of the spikes generated by each stimulation (Figure 2). Subsequently, we investigated the channels that could be used to identify a spike pattern that was stimulated by two different Stch (see Section 2.2). Henceforth, we will refer to such a channel as Identifiable. The classification method was the BPNN machine learning method, similar to that described in [25].
If a learning process occurs in the neuronal network, the number of Identifiable channels that can identify a spike pattern stimulated by different channels must be increased by repeated stimulation. Therefore, we analyzed the variation in the number of continuous Identifiable channels after repeated stimulation in each channel, as follows. First, we set an Analyzing Window including four stimulations in a time series (see Figure 3(a)) and counted the number of consecutive Identifiable channels in this window. For example, the number of consecutive Identifiable channels was 2 in Figure 3(a). Next, these procedures were repeated by sliding the Analyzing Window in the direction of the temporal axis (by stimulation). The outline of this procedure is shown in Figure 3(b).
Figure 4 shows the variation of consecutive Identifiable channels with time (see Section 2.3) in each channel in culture 07 when stch was ch36 and ch54 and SI was 1.5 s. The horizonal axis represents the position of the analyzing window, which corresponds to the timing of the stimulation. The vertical axis represents the number of consecutive Identifiable channels. The results depicted in this figure showed that the number of consecutive Identifiable channels increased in some channels.
To investigate the spatial distribution of such channels, we colored cells in yellow and red (the meaning of these colors is described in the caption of this figure). Channels for which an increment in the number of consecutive Identifiable channels was observed (termed yellow cells henceforth) gathered in a particular area, which was comparatively wide. In contrast, a decrement in the number of consecutive Identifiable channels (termed red cells henceforth) was also observed in some channels; however, the number of red cells was much smaller than that of yellow cells.
We also applied this process to other cultures and all combinations of Stch (St11 vs. St36, St36 vs. St54, and St11 vs. St54) at an SI of 0.5 or 1.5 s. Figure 5 shows the distribution of yellow cells and red cell in these cultures when SI was 1.5 s. Because of space constraints, the time variation of consecutive Identifiable channels, as shown in Figure 4, was omitted. Although similar results were observed among these cultures, there was variation in culture and stimulation channel. Incidentally, the results obtained for cultures 01, 03, and 06 were not shown, as neither yellow cells nor red cells were observed in these cultures. We considered that these results depended on the individual properties of each cell, including the distribution of synaptic weight, refractory period, etc.
Figure 6 shows the distribution of yellow cells and red cells in these cultures when SI was 0.5 s. For this SI, yellow cells and red cells were observed only in cultures 07, 09, and 11. Moreover, the number of these cells was smaller than that observed for the SI of 1.5 s, even in these cultures (compare Figure 6 with Figure 5).
As an explanation for these results, it is possible that, for short stimulation periods, a few spikes resulting by the previous stimulation remain and overlap with the new spike, thus interfering with each other (see Figure 7).
Although repeated stimulation led to an increase in the number of yellow cells, it is unlikely that these phenomena happened accidentally. To address this issue, we performed the same analysis for imitation spike trains generated by random numbers. Few yellow cells and red cells were detected, which demonstrated that the results obtained for the cultures were not accidental.
Some learning processes are aimed at improving the identification of communications in a neuronal network via repeated stimulation in a particular area of the network. These learning processes are affected by the individuality of each cell. Although the reason for the existence of red cells (against our assumption) has not been clarified, it is possible that they are inhibitory neurons. Moreover, the effect of SI on the learning process is not clear, as we did not perform experiments using an SI longer than 1.5 s; however, it seems that the effect of the learning process was weakened, as the condition of the neuronal networks (distribution of synaptic weight, etc.) were reset before the stimulations.
We consider that these learning processes are similar to a repeated training aimed at learning information, to the skills of exercise, to learning how to play musical instruments, etc.
Our recent studies showed that stimulated channels were able to identify various spatiotemporal patterns of spike wave propagation in specific areas of the neuronal network. Here, we report that repeated stimulation increased the number of consecutive Identifiable channels at an SI of 1.5 s in a cultured neuronal network. This suggests that part of the learning process (learning knowledge or skill in view of basic brain activity) occurs via repetition training (stimulation). In contrast, this effect was barely observed for an SI of 0.5 s, because a few of the spikes elicited by the previous stimulation remain and overlap with the new spike, thus interfering with each other. As described above, spike repetition at an interval of 1.5 s yielded a superior result compared with an interval of 0.5 s for the identification of consecutive Identifiable channels. However, the effect of an SI longer than 1.5 s on the learning process has not been assessed. In future studies, we will examine the relationship between SI and learning process more clearly and identify the SI that is most suitable for the learning process. Moreover, we will investigate the learning process from the perspective of brain activity in greater detail.
[1] |
Bonifazi P, Goldin M, Picardo MA, et al. (2009) GABAergic hub neurons orchestrate synchrony in developing hippocampal networks. Science 326: 1419–1424. doi: 10.1126/science.1175509
![]() |
[2] | Kamimura T, Nakamura K, Yoneda K, et al. (2010) Information communication in brain based on memory loop neural circuit. ICIS2010 & SEDM2010 (appears in IEEE Xplore), Chengdu, June, 23–25. |
[3] | Choe Y (2002) Analogical Cascade: A theory on the role of the thalamo-cortical loop in brain function. Neurocomputing 52: 713–719. |
[4] | Tamura S, Mizuno-Matsumoto Y, Chen YW, et al. (2009) Association and abstraction on neural circuit loop and coding. IIHMSP 2009 10-07: 546–549 (appears in IEEE Xplore). |
[5] |
Thorpre S, Fize D, Marlot C (1996) Speed of processing in the human visual system. Nature 381: 520. doi: 10.1038/381520a0
![]() |
[6] |
Cessac B, Paugam-Moisy H, Viéville T (2010) Overview of facts and issues about neural coding by spike. J Physiol-Paris 104: 5–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2009.11.002
![]() |
[7] | Kliper O, Horn D, Quenet B, et al. (2004) Analysis of spatiotemporal patterns in a model of olfaction. Neurocomputing 58: 1027–1032. |
[8] |
Fujita K, Kashimori Y, Kambara T (2007) Spatiotemporal burst coding for extracting features of spatiotemporally varying stimuli. Biol Cybern 97: 293–305. doi: 10.1007/s00422-007-0175-z
![]() |
[9] |
Tyukin I, Tyukina T, Van LC (2009) Invariant template matching in systems with spatiotemporal coding: A matter of instability. Neural Networks 22: 425–449. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2009.01.014
![]() |
[10] |
Mohemmed A, Schliebs S, Matsuda S, et al. (2013) Training spiking neural networks to associate spatio-temporal input–output spike patterns. Neurocomputing 107: 3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neucom.2012.08.034
![]() |
[11] |
Olshausen BA, Field DJ (1996) Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images. Nature 381: 607–609. doi: 10.1038/381607a0
![]() |
[12] |
Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ (1997) The "independent components" of natural scenes are edge filters. Vision Res 37: 3327–3338. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00121-1
![]() |
[13] | Aviel Y, Horn D, Abeles M (2004) Synfire waves in small balanced networks. Neurocomputing 58–60: 123–127. |
[14] | Abeles M (1982) Local Cortical Circuits: An Electrophysiological study, Berlin: Springer. |
[15] | Shinozaki T, Câteau H, Urakubo H, et al. (2007) Controlling synfire chain by inhibitory synaptic input. J Phys Soc Jpn 76: 044806. |
[16] | Izhikevich EM (2014) Polychronization: Computation with spikes. Neural Comput 18: 245–282. |
[17] |
Perc M (2007) Fluctuating excitability: A mechanism for self-sustained information flow in excite arrays. Chaos Soliton Fract 32: 1118–1124. doi: 10.1016/j.chaos.2005.11.035
![]() |
[18] |
Zhang H, Wang Q, Perc M, et al. (2013) Synaptic plasticity induced transition of spike propagation in neuronal networks. Commun Nonlinear Sci 18: 601–615. doi: 10.1016/j.cnsns.2012.08.009
![]() |
[19] |
Mizuno-Matsumoto Y, Okazaki K, Kato A, et al. (1999) Visualization of epileptogenic phenomena using crosscorrelation analysis: Localization of epileptic foci and propagation of epileptiform discharges. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 46: 271–279. doi: 10.1109/10.748980
![]() |
[20] |
Mizuno-Matsumoto Y, Ishijima M, Shinosaki K, et al. (2001) Transient global amnesia (TGA) in a MEG study. Brain Topogr 13: 269–274. doi: 10.1023/A:1011176612377
![]() |
[21] | Nishitani Y, Hosokawa C, Mizuno-Matsumoto Y, et al. (2012) Detection of M-sequences from spike sequence in neuronal networks. Comput Intell Neurosci 2012: 167–185. |
[22] | Nishitani Y, Hosokawa C, Mizuno-Matsumoto Y, et al. (2014) Synchronized code sequences from spike trains in cultured neuronal networks. Int J Eng Ind 5: 13–24. |
[23] | Tamura S, Nishitani Y, Hosokawa C, et al. (2016) Simulation of code spectrum and code flow of cultured neuronal networks. Comput Intell Neurosci 2016: 1–12. |
[24] |
Nishitani Y, Hosokawa C, Mizuno-Matsumoto Y, et al. (2016) Classification of spike wave propagations in a cultured neuronal network: Investigating a brain communication mechanism. AIMS Neurosci 4: 1–13. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2017.1.1
![]() |
[25] | Nishitani Y, Hosokawa C, Mizuno-Matsumoto Y, et al. (2017) Effect of correlating adjacent neurons for identifying communications: Feasibility experiment in a cultured neuronal network AIMS Neurosci 5: 18–31. |
[26] | Ando H (2002) Human Brain Regions Involved in Visual Motion Prediction. Neuro Image Human Brain Mapping 2002 Meeting, 599. |
[27] |
Rumelhart DE, Hinton GE, Williams RJ (1986) Learning representations by back-propagating errors. Nature 323: 533–536. doi: 10.1038/323533a0
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |