Loading [Contrib]/a11y/accessibility-menu.js
Commentary Special Issues

Learning-Related Synaptic Reconfiguration in Hippocampal Networks: Memory Storage or Waveguide Tuning?

  • A fundamental assumption of current hypotheses regarding hippocampal involvement in memory formation is that changes in synaptic connections between hippocampal neurons serve to encode information about recent events. An alternative possibility is that synaptic changes are “ruts” left by dynamic traveling waves involved in memory processes rather than a mechanism for storing memories of events. Specifically, traveling waves of activity in corticohippocampal circuits may sometimes modify those circuits as they propagate through them, thereby changing the paths and qualities of future wave patterns.

    Citation: Eduardo Mercado III. Learning-Related Synaptic Reconfiguration in Hippocampal Networks: Memory Storage or Waveguide Tuning?[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2015, 2(1): 28-34. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.1.28

    Related Papers:

  • A fundamental assumption of current hypotheses regarding hippocampal involvement in memory formation is that changes in synaptic connections between hippocampal neurons serve to encode information about recent events. An alternative possibility is that synaptic changes are “ruts” left by dynamic traveling waves involved in memory processes rather than a mechanism for storing memories of events. Specifically, traveling waves of activity in corticohippocampal circuits may sometimes modify those circuits as they propagate through them, thereby changing the paths and qualities of future wave patterns.


    1. Synaptic plasticity as a mechanism for hippocampal encoding of events

    Half a century after long-term potentiation (LTP) was first observed in the rabbit hippocampus [1], researchers continue to uncover new facets of this phenomenon. The specific role that LTP plays in hippocampal-dependent learning and memory processes has been extensively debated [5,6]. Despite continuing uncertainty about exactly how hippocampal circuits contribute to different memory abilities [7], most researchers no longer question that hippocampal LTP reflects changes in synaptic strength that serve to encode experienced events or associations between such events [8]. The research and hypotheses described by Mitsushima [8,9] follow in this tradition by showing that rapid avoidance conditioning leads to selective strengthening of synapses within hippocampal circuits, and by suggesting that these synaptic changes constitute a memory trace for the events experienced during training.

    Mitsushima [8,9] reports that both excitatory and inhibitory synapses on CA1 neurons in rats were strengthened by a training experience in which a shock was paired with a behavior that would normally be reinforcing (moving from a lighted area to a darker one). These synaptic changes were correlated with behavioral changes,indicating that rats learned from this experience. When the synaptic changes were prevented from occurring,training did not change rats’ behavior. Together,these findings show that when rats are learning,their hippocampal connections are changing,and that interfering with hippocampal processing can disrupt avoidance learning. Mitsushima also notes that there was considerable variability in terms of how synapses changed at any given neuron,and that only a subset of hippocampal neurons (in the dorsal region) showed such changes. These observations are consistent with past reports implicating hippocampal LTP in learning [10,11].

    2. Cholinergic modulation as a trigger for memory storage

    One novel aspect of the studies by Mitsushima [8] is the measurement of acetylcholine (ACh) levels in CA1 before,during,and after conditioning. Less than a minute of training led to significant increases in hippocampal ACh levels. Surprisingly,levels remained elevated for an hour or more after training. Allowing rats to explore a chamber or mildly shocking rats in the chamber led to increases in ACh levels that lasted less than 15 min. Mitsushima [8,9] speculates that long-lasting increases in ACh levels in the hippocampus triggered learning-related LTP. This interpretation is indirectly supported by studies showing that controlling cholinergic levels in auditory cortex during sound presentations can enhance experience-dependent changes in cortical sensitivities [12,13]. ACh-dependent cortical plasticity can be attributed,at least in part,to changes in excitatory and inhibitory synapses within cortical circuits [14,15]. Increases in ACh levels within the hippocampus may be accompanied by increases in A Ch levels within cortical networks [16],which could in turn affect qualities of the cortical activity patterns that drive hippocampal activity. Thus,learning-related changes in hippocampal synapses might depend on both heightened cholinergic levels in CA1 and changes to incoming activity patterns.

    Cholinergic modulation could potentially contribute to learning and memory processes through several different corticohippocampal mechanisms [17]. One issue not directly addressed by Mitsushima [8] is what determines the amount and duration of ACh release into the hippocampus by medial septum neurons. The activity of these neurons may be partly determined by hippocampal activity [18,19]. Specifically,hippocampal circuits could drive activity in the medial septum,thereby controlling which experiences lead to large changes in synapses within CA1. Another feature of cholinergic modulation that is unclear from Mitsushima’s microdialysis measures is whether tonic changes in ACh levels drove the observed synaptic changes in CA1,or whether these changes resulted from more phasic ACh release [20]. Mitsushima notes that electrophysiological recordings in CA1 show intermittent bursts of high-frequency ripples after training. Increased cholinergic levels should suppress such firing patterns [21],suggesting that transient rather than constant changes in medial septum activity may be responsible for the observed increases in cholinergic levels.

    3. Effects of cholinergic modulation on corticohippocampal traveling waves

    Numerous lines of evidence,including the findings reviewed by Mitsushima [8],show that: (1) learning experiences can lead to increases and decreases in the strength of excitatory and inhibitory connections between neurons; and (2) manipulations that disrupt synaptic plasticity processes can disrupt learning and memory performances. Such findings have led many researchers to conclude that memories are encoded in the hippocampus as sets of synaptic strengths. Other evidence raises questions,however,about whether synaptic modifications should be viewed as a memory storage mechanism. First,many of the changes observed in synapses,including a subset of those described by Mitsushima,dissipate more quickly than the behavioral changes produced by training. Second,evidence from motor system research suggests that synaptic strengths can continuously fluctuate without affecting recall [22]. Observations of dendritic spine turnover in neural circuits have similarly led some to suggest that dynamic configurations of connections between neurons rather than fixed changes in connectivity may give rise to stable memories [23,24,25,26]. Finally,recent work implicating glial function in both sensory [27] and motor plasticity [28] in cortical networks,suggests that learning-related changes in behavior are probably not simply a function of changing connectivity between neurons.

    How might dynamic constellations of synaptic strengths contribute to learning if not by encoding memories? Ajemian and colleagues [22] proposed that dynamic patterns of connections might fluctuate coherently,preserving static input-output mappings. Routtenberg [25]hypothesized that memory traces are constantly being duplicated such that memories are preserved despite the impermanence of synaptic changes. Both of these explanations replace static synaptic memory traces with persistent synaptic “swarms” that play an equivalent functional role (see also [4]). An alternative possibility is that neither individual synapses nor dynamic sets of synapses serve to encode and store memories. Instead,synaptic dynamics and other forms of neural and glial plasticity may be a side effect of memory processes rather than a mechanism for representing either past events or associations. Consideration of the properties and correlates of traveling waves of neural activity within the hippocampus and cortex provides important clues about how and why such waves might drive synaptic changes even if synapses are not functioning as memory traces.

    Increases in ACh levels like those reported by Mitsushima [8] after training are associated with increased hippocampal theta activity [17,20]. Recent studies have shown that theta waves propagate in predictable directions and at predictable rates within the hippocampus [29,30],and that concurrent activity in cortical networks is often phase locked to hippocampal theta [31,32,33]. In some cases,theta activity within cortical networks appears to propagate from hippocampal circuits via volume conduction [33]. Theta activity in the hippocampus [17,34] and cortex [35] has often been associated with learning processes. Collectively,these findings predict that traveling waves in cortical and hippocampal networks are likely to be spatiotemporally coordinated [36],and to be more prevalent after experiences that increase ACh levels. Several current theories of hippocampal function suggest that coordinated,recurrent waves of activity that occur within and across hippocampal and cortical circuits should lead to significant changes in synaptic connections [4,37,38]. Computational simulations of retinal waves show that variations in extracellular levels of ACh can affect when traveling waves arise as well as their qualities [39]; cholinergic levels in the hippocampus might similarly influence the prevalence and properties of traveling theta waves.

    4. Effects of synaptic plasticity on wave propagation

    Historically,hippocampal theta activity has been thought to contribute to learning by coordinating information transfer and encoding [17,38],thereby facilitating memory trace formation via synaptic plasticity in hippocampal circuits. The fact that theta-related modes of processing in the hippocampus are associated with synaptic changes does not,however,imply that such changes encode records of experienced events that will later be reactivated to recreate internal states associated with those experiences. Instead,learning-related changes in cortical and hippocampal synapses may change how traveling waves propagate through these circuits [40,41]. In Pavlov’s cortical theory of conditioned response formation,learning-related changes in neural connections modulate the qualities of traveling wave patterns in cortex. Similarly,synaptic changes in hippocampal circuits could affect patterns of theta wave propagation,modulating the rate,direction,or strength of these traveling waves.

    Few researchers have studied the effects of synaptic changes on the qualities of traveling waves within the brain. However,in developing animals there is clear evidence that changes in the strengths of both excitatory and inhibitory synapses can lead to variations in wave propagation [42,43],and vice versa [44]. Studies of compensatory plasticity also suggest that synaptic changes affect how cortical waves propagate [45]. Recurrent,traveling corticohippocampal waves,or states associated with those waves such as elevated cholinergic levels,may lead to changes in synaptic connections [46],which in turn may affect how subsequent waves propagate. These synaptic changes might then be viewed as a consequence of traveling wave propagation that can potentially affect future activity patterns (e.g.,trajectories),without synapses storing any specific information about the events that initiated those waves. Under this interpretation,the effects of propagating corticohippocampal waves on syna ptic strengths are loosely analogous to the effects of ocean waves on the configuration of sand along a shoreline. The activity of different wave patterns might “shift the sands” in predictable ways,but which specific synapses are affected may be less relevant to the neural encoding of events than the overall effects of these changes on future wave propagation [47]. Dynamic changes in synaptic strengths might reflect the variability of wave patterns rather than encoding of unique events. This interpretation is similar to Miller’s (1989) proposal that self-organizing corticohippocampal loops resonate at particular frequencies (e.g.,theta) to encode the environment,and that the spatiotemporal distribution of theta-related activity becomes shifted during learning. In this scenario,the qualities of waves rather than the strengths of individual neural connections encode events [48,49].

    5. Conclusion

    Experiences that quickly generate long-lasting changes in behavior,such as the inhibitory avoidance task used by Mitsushima [8],undoubtedly are associated with complex,temporally dynamic changes in neuromodulatory activity and synaptic connections in multiple brain regions. A major challenge in learning and memory research is to understand what functions (if any) such chemical and structural changes serve. Although many findings are consistent with the hypothesis that synapses within the hippocampus function as engrams of varying longevity,it remains possible that the specific changes in synaptic strengths caused by learning experiences are less critical to determining the contents of representations of past experiences than is generally assumed.

    Acknowledgments

    This work was supported in part by NSF grant #SBE-0542013 to the Temporal Dynamics of Learning Center, an NSF Science of Learning Center.

    Conflict of Interest

    The author declares no conflicts of interest in this paper.

    [1] Lomo T (1966) Frequency potentiation of excitatory synaptic activity in the dentate area of the hippocampal formation. Acta Physiol Scand 68: 128.
    [2] Shors TJ, Matzel LD (1997) Long-term potentiation: What's learning got to do with it? Behav Brain Sci 20: 597-614;
    [3] Baudry M, Bi X, Gall C, et al. (2011) The biochemistry of memory: The 26 year journey of a 'new and specific hypothesis'. Neurobiol Learn Mem 95: 125-133. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.11.015
    [4] Buzsaki G (2010) Neural syntax: Cell assemblies, synapsembles, and readers. Neuron 68:362-385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.023
    [5] Squire LR, Wixted JT (2011) The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since H.M. Annu Rev Neurosci 34: 259-288. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113720
    [6] Rissman J, Wagner AD (2012) Distributed representations in memory: Insights from functional brain imaging. Annu Rev Psychol 63: 101-128. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100344
    [7] Rudy JW (2014) The neurobiology of learning and memory. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
    [8] Mitsushima D (2015) Contextual learning requires functional diversity at excitatory and inhibitory synapses onto CA1 pyramidal neurons. AIMS Neurosci 2: 7-17. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.1.7
    [9] Mitsushima D, Sano A, Takahashi T (2013) A cholinergic trigger drives learning-induced plasticity at hippocampal synapses. Nat Commun 4: 2760.
    [10] Takeuchi T, Duszkiewicz AJ, Morris RG (2014) The synaptic plasticity and memory hypothesis: Encoding, storage and persistence. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 369: 20130288.
    [11] Whitlock JR, Heynen AJ, Shuler MG, et al. (2006) Learning induces long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Science 313: 1093-1097. doi: 10.1126/science.1128134
    [12] Bakin JS, Weinberger NM (1996) Induction of a physiological memory in the cerebral cortex by stimulation of the nucleus basalis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93: 11219-11224. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.20.11219
    [13] Mercado E, III, Bao S, Orduna I, et al. (2001) Basal forebrain stimulation changes cortical sensitivities to complex sound. Neuroreport 12: 2283-2287. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200107200-00047
    [14] Froemke RC, Carcea I, Barker AJ, et al. (2013) Long-term modification of cortical synapses improves sensory perception. Nat Neurosci 16: 79-88.
    [15] Froemke RC, Merzenich MM, Schreiner CE (2007) A synaptic memory trace for cortical receptive field plasticity. Nature 450: 425-429. doi: 10.1038/nature06289
    [16] Marrosu F, Portas C, Mascia MS, et al. (1995) Microdialysis measurement of cortical and hippocampal acetylcholine release during sleep-wake cycle in freely moving cats. Brain Res 671:329-332. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)01399-3
    [17] Newman EL, Gupta K, Climer JR, et al. (2012) Cholinergic modulation of cognitive processing: Insights drawn from computational models. Front Behav Neurosci 6: 24.
    [18] Rokers B, Mercado E, III, Allen MT, et al. (2002) A connectionist model of septohippocampal dynamics during conditioning: Closing the loop. Behav Neurosci 116: 48-62. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.116.1.48
    [19] Manseau F, Goutagny R, Danik M, et al. (2008) The hippocamposeptal pathway generates rhythmic firing of GABAergic neurons in the medial septum and diagonal bands: An investigation using a complete septohippocampal preparation in vitro. J Neurosci 28: 4096-4107. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0247-08.2008
    [20] Zhang H, Lin SC, Nicolelis MA (2010) Spatiotemporal coupling between hippocampal acetylcholine release and theta oscillations in vivo. J Neurosci 30: 13431-13440. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1144-10.2010
    [21] Vandecasteele M, Varga V, Berenyi A, et al. (2014) Optogenetic activation of septal cholinergic neurons suppresses sharp wave ripples and enhances theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111: 13535-13540. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1411233111
    [22] Ajemian R, D'Ausilio A, Moorman H, et al. (2013) A theory for how sensorimotor skills are learned and retained in noisy and nonstationary neural circuits. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110: E5078-5087. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1320116110
    [23] Routtenberg A (2013) Lifetime memories from persistently supple synapses. Hippocampus 23:202-206. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22088
    [24] Routtenberg A (2008) Long-lasting memory from evanescent networks. Eur J Pharmacol 585:60-63. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2008.02.047
    [25] Routtenberg A (2008) The substrate for long-lasting memory: If not protein synthesis, then what? Neurobiol Learn Mem 89: 225-233. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.10.012
    [26] Richards BA, Frankland PW (2013) The conjunctive trace. Hippocampus 23: 207-212. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22089
    [27] Chen N, Sugihara H, Sharma J, et al. (2012) Nucleus basalis-enabled stimulus-specific plasticity in the visual cortex is mediated by astrocytes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109: E2832-2841. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206557109
    [28] McKenzie IA, Ohayon D, Li H, et al. (2014) Motor skill learning requires active central myelination. Science 346: 318-322. doi: 10.1126/science.1254960
    [29] Patel J, Fujisawa S, Berenyi A, et al. (2012) Traveling theta waves along the entire septotemporal axis of the hippocampus. Neuron 75: 410-417. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.015
    [30] Lubenov EV, Siapas AG (2009) Hippocampal theta oscillations are travelling waves. Nature 459:534-539. doi: 10.1038/nature08010
    [31] Benchenane K, Peyrache A, Khamassi M, et al. (2010) Coherent theta oscillations and reorganization of spike timing in the hippocampal- prefrontal network upon learning. Neuron 66:921-936. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.013
    [32] Siapas AG, Lubenov EV, Wilson MA (2005) Prefrontal phase locking to hippocampal theta oscillations. Neuron 46: 141-151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.028
    [33] Sirota A, Montgomery S, Fujisawa S, et al. (2008) Entrainment of neocortical neurons and gamma oscillations by the hippocampal theta rhythm. Neuron 60: 683-697. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.014
    [34] Buzsaki G (2005) Theta rhythm of navigation: Link between path integration and landmark navigation, episodic and semantic memory. Hippocampus 15: 827-840. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20113
    [35] Landfield PW, McGaugh JL, Tusa RJ (1972) Theta rhythm: A temporal correlate of memory storage processes in the rat. Science 175: 87-89. doi: 10.1126/science.175.4017.87
    [36] Miller R (1989) Cortico-hippocampal interplay: Self-organizing phase-locked loops for indexing memory. Psychobiol 17: 115-128.
    [37] Palmer JH, Gong P (2014) Associative learning of classical conditioning as an emergent property of spatially extended spiking neural circuits with synaptic plasticity. Front Comput Neurosci 8: 79.
    [38] Battaglia FP, Benchenane K, Sirota A, et al. (2011) The hippocampus: Hub of brain network communication for memory. Trends Cogn Sci 15: 310-318.
    [39] Lansdell B, Ford K, Kutz JN (2014) A reaction-diffusion model of cholinergic retinal waves.PLoS Comput Biol 10: e1003953. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003953
    [40] Wester JC, Contreras D (2012) Columnar interactions determine horizontal propagation of recurrent network activity in neocortex. J Neurosci 32: 5454-5471. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5006-11.2012
    [41] Pavlov IP (1927) Conditioned reflexes. London: Oxford University Press.
    [42] Griffen TC, Wang L, Fontanini A, et al. (2012) Developmental regulation of spatio-temporal patterns of cortical circuit activation. Front Cell Neurosci 6: 65.
    [43] Wang L, Fontanini A, Maffei A (2011) Visual experience modulates spatio-temporal dynamics of circuit activation. Front Cell Neurosci 5: 12.
    [44] Blankenship AG, Feller MB (2010) Mechanisms underlying spontaneous patterned activity in developing neural circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci 11: 18-29. doi: 10.1038/nrn2759
    [45] Palagina G, Eysel UT, Jancke D (2009) Strengthening of lateral activation in adult rat visual cortex after retinal lesions captured with voltage-sensitive dye imaging in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 8743-8747. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900068106
    [46] Ponulak F, Hopfield JJ (2013) Rapid, parallel path planning by propagating wavefronts of spiking neural activity. Front Comput Neurosci 7: 98.
    [47] Mercado E, III (2014) Relating cortical wave dynamics to learning and remembering. AIMS Neurosci 1: 185-209. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2014.3.185
    [48] Heitmann S, Gong P, Breakspear M (2012) A computational role for bistability and traveling waves in motor cortex. Front Comput Neurosci 6: 67.
    [49] Gong P, van Leeuwen C (2009) Distributed dynamical computation in neural circuits with propagating coherent activity patterns. PLoS Comput Biol 5: e1000611. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000611
  • Reader Comments
  • © 2015 the Author(s), licensee AIMS Press. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0)
通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
  • 1. 

    沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

  1. 本站搜索
  2. 百度学术搜索
  3. 万方数据库搜索
  4. CNKI搜索

Metrics

Article views(5053) PDF downloads(886) Cited by(0)

Article outline

Other Articles By Authors

/

DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
Return
Return

Catalog