Research article

ERPs to Alcohol Images among Hispanic and Non-Hispanic Female College Freshmen

  • Received: 25 August 2015 Accepted: 16 December 2015 Published: 27 December 2015
  • Research suggests that young women, particularly Latinas, may be at risk for problem drinking during the home-to-college transition. In this study, we used ERP cue-reactivity to explore physiological correlates of alcohol use and expectancies across the freshman year in Hispanic (H) and Non-Hispanic White (N-H) women. In the fall (t1) and spring (t2) semesters of their freshman year of college, 40 women (16 H) reported alcohol use and expectancies. At each session, N200 and P300 ERPs were elicited by two oddball tasks (counterbalanced within session): 1) to detect alcohol targets while ignoring control items (household object distracters) and frequently presented nonsense shapes and 2) to detect object targets while ignoring alcohol distracters and nonsense shapes. P300 amplitude was larger for targets (versus non-targets) and for alcohol images (versus control images), but did not change over time or differ by ethnicity. P300 latency results included time x target and ethnicity x target interactions. Latency differences for target images were attenuated at t2, and N-Hs were more reactive to stimuli classed as targets regardless of whether these depicted alcohol or control images. N200s had higher amplitude and longer latency at t2, suggesting a change with acclimation to the college setting, but did not differ by target status, image type or ethnic group. P300 latency was positively correlated with the personalismo subscale of acculturation indicating that individuals with more social, people-oriented personalities were more distracted by alcohol images when they appeared as non-targets. N200 amplitude was correlated with positive alcohol expectancies, and this pattern changed over time (t1 versus t2), suggesting subtle, expectancy-related changes in alcohol processing as students acclimated to the college setting. Taken together, these results suggest that the cue-reactivity paradigm described here may be a useful tool for examining subtle physiological correlates of college drinking.

    Citation: Natalie Ceballos, Abigail Mayfield, Reiko Graham. ERPs to Alcohol Images among Hispanic and Non-Hispanic Female College Freshmen[J]. AIMS Neuroscience, 2016, 3(1): 1-21. doi: 10.3934/Neuroscience.2016.1.1

    Related Papers:

  • Research suggests that young women, particularly Latinas, may be at risk for problem drinking during the home-to-college transition. In this study, we used ERP cue-reactivity to explore physiological correlates of alcohol use and expectancies across the freshman year in Hispanic (H) and Non-Hispanic White (N-H) women. In the fall (t1) and spring (t2) semesters of their freshman year of college, 40 women (16 H) reported alcohol use and expectancies. At each session, N200 and P300 ERPs were elicited by two oddball tasks (counterbalanced within session): 1) to detect alcohol targets while ignoring control items (household object distracters) and frequently presented nonsense shapes and 2) to detect object targets while ignoring alcohol distracters and nonsense shapes. P300 amplitude was larger for targets (versus non-targets) and for alcohol images (versus control images), but did not change over time or differ by ethnicity. P300 latency results included time x target and ethnicity x target interactions. Latency differences for target images were attenuated at t2, and N-Hs were more reactive to stimuli classed as targets regardless of whether these depicted alcohol or control images. N200s had higher amplitude and longer latency at t2, suggesting a change with acclimation to the college setting, but did not differ by target status, image type or ethnic group. P300 latency was positively correlated with the personalismo subscale of acculturation indicating that individuals with more social, people-oriented personalities were more distracted by alcohol images when they appeared as non-targets. N200 amplitude was correlated with positive alcohol expectancies, and this pattern changed over time (t1 versus t2), suggesting subtle, expectancy-related changes in alcohol processing as students acclimated to the college setting. Taken together, these results suggest that the cue-reactivity paradigm described here may be a useful tool for examining subtle physiological correlates of college drinking.


    加载中
    [1] Carter DF, Locks AM, Winkle-Wagner R (2013) From when and where I enter: theoretical and empirical considerations of minority students’ transition to college, In: Paulsen, M.B. Editor, Higher Education: Handbook of Theory and Research, New York: Springer, 93-149.
    [2] Tinto V (1987) Leaving college: rethinking the causes and cures of student attrition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    [3] Ward L, Siegel MJ, Davenport Z (2012) First-generation college students: understanding and improving the experience from recruitment to commencement. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
    [4] Cano MA, Castillo LG (2010) The role of enculturation and acculturation on Latina college student distress. J Hispanic Higher Ed 9: 221-231.
    [5] Orbe MP, Groscurth CR (2004) A co-cultural theoretical analysis of communicating on campus and at home: exploring the negotiation strategies of first generation college (FGC) students. Qual Res Rep Com 5: 41-47.
    [6] O’Malley PM, Johnston LD (2002) Epidemiology of alcohol and other drug use among American college students. J Stud Alcohol Suppl 14: 23-39.
    [7] Del Boca FK, Darkes J, Greenbaum PE, et al. (2004) Up close and personal: temporal variability in the drinking of individual college students during their first year. J Consult Clin Psychol 72: 155-164. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.155
    [8] Ham LS, Hope DA (2003) College students and problematic drinking: a review of the literature. Clin Psychol Rev 23: 719-759.
    [9] Fairlie AM, Maggs JL, Lanza ST (2015) Prepartying, drinking games, and extreme drinking among college students: a daily-level investigation. Addict Behav 42: 91-95. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.11.001
    [10] Windle M (2003) Alcohol use among adolescents and young adults. Alcohol Res Health 27:79-85.
    [11] Blume SB (1992) Alcohol and other drug problems in women, In: Lowinson, J.H., Ruiz, P. Editors, Substance Abuse: A Comprehensive Textbook, 2 Ed., Maryland: William and Wilkins, 794-807.
    [12] Cloninger RC, Christiansen KO, Reich T, et al. (1978) Implications of sex differences in the prevalence of antisocial personality, alcoholism and criminality for familial transmission. Arch Gen Psychiatry 35: 719-759.
    [13] Keyes KM, Grant BF, Hasin DS (2008) Evidence for a closing gender gap in alcohol use, abuse, and dependence in the United States population. Drug Alcohol Depend 93: 21-29. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.08.017
    [14] Corbin WR, Vaughan EL, Fromme K (2008) Ethnic differences and the closing of the sex gap in alcohol use among college-bound students. Psychol Addict Behav 22: 240-248.
    [15] Cardenas V, Kerby S (2012) The state of Latinos in the United States. Washington, D.C.: Center for American Progress.
    [16] Zemore SE (2012) The effect of social desirability on reported motivation, substance use severity, and treatment attendance. J Subst Abuse Treat 42: 400-412. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2011.09.013
    [17] Robinson TE, Berridge KC, (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev 18: 247-291. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-P
    [18] Parsons OA (1998) Neurocognitive deficits in alcoholics and social drinkers: a continuum? Alcohol Clin Exp Res 22: 954-961.
    [19] Parsons OA, Nixon SJ (1998) Cognitive functioning in sober social drinkers: a review of the research since 1986. J Stud Alcohol 59: 180-190. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1998.59.180
    [20] Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, et al. (2007) Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: a review and model. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 86: 263-283.
    [21] Wiers RW, Boelema SR, Nikolaou K, et al. (2015) On the development of implicit and control processes in relation to substance use in adolescence. Curr Addict Rep 2: 141-155.
    [22] Stacy AW, Wiers (2010) Implicit cognition and addiction: a tool for explaining paradoxical behavior. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 6: 551-575. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131444
    [23] Field M, Cox WM (2008) Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend 97: 1-20.
    [24] Cox WM, Pothos EM, Hosier SG (2007) Cognitive-motivational predictors of excessive drinkers’ success in changing. Psychopharmacology 192: 499-510.
    [25] Field M, Mogg K, Zetteler J, et al. (2004) Attentional biases for alcohol cues in heavy and light social drinkers: the roles of initial orienting and maintained attention. Psychopharmacology 176:88-93.
    [26] Jones BT, Bruce G, Livingstone S, et al. (2006) Alcohol-related attentional bias in problem drinkers with the flicker change blindness paradigm. Psychol Addict Behav 20: 171-177.
    [27] Townshend JM, Duka T (2007) Avoidance of alcohol-related stimuli in alcohol-dependent inpatients. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 31: 1349-1357.
    [28] Warren CA, McDonough BE (1999) Event-related brain potentials as indicators of smoking cue-reactivity. Clin Neurophysiol 110:1570-1584.
    [29] Prabhu VR, Porjesz B, Chorlian DB, et al. (2001) Visual P3 in female alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 25: 531-539. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2001.tb02247.x
    [30] Realmuto G, Begleiter H, Odencrantz J, et al. (1993) Event-related potential evidence of dysfunction in automatic processing in abstinent alcoholics. Biol Psychiatry 33: 594-601. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90097-W
    [31] Hesselbrock V, Begleiter H, Porjesz B, et al. (2001) P300 event-related potential amplitude as an endophenotype of alcoholism—evidence from the collaborative study on the genetics of alcoholism. J Biomed Sci 8: 77-82.
    [32] Singh SM, Basu D (2009) The P300 event-related potential and its possible role as an endophenotype for studying substance use disorders: a review. Addict Biol 14: 298-309. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00124.x
    [33] Euser AS, Arends LR, Evans BE, et al. (2012) The P300 event-related brain potential as a neurobiological endophenotype for substance use disorders: a meta-analytic investigation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36: 572-603.
    [34] Polich J, Criado JR (2006) Neuropsychology and neuropharmacology of P3a and P3b. Int J Psychophysiol 60: 172-185.
    [35] Nixon SJ, Paul R, Phillips M (1998) Cognitive efficiency in alcoholics and polysubstance abusers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 22: 1414-1420. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb03929.x
    [36] Herrmann MJ, Weijers HG, Wiesbeck GA, et al. (2001) Alcohol cue-reactivity in heavy and light social drinkers as revealed by event-related potentials. Alcohol Alcohol 36: 588-593. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/36.6.588
    [37] Bartholow BD, Henry EA, Lust SA (2007) Effects of alcohol sensitivity on P3 event-related potential reactivity to alcohol cues. Psychol Addict Behav 21: 555-563. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.4.555
    [38] Ceballos NA, Giuliano RJ, Wicha NY, et al. (2012) Acute stress and event-related potential correlates of attention to alcohol images in social drinkers. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 73: 761-771. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.761
    [39] Delplanque S, Silvert L, Hot P, et al. (2006) Arousal and valence effects on event-related P3a and P3b during emotional categorization. Int J Psychophysiol 60: 315-322. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.06.006
    [40] Petit G, Kornreich C, Maurage P, et al. (2012) Early attentional modulation by alcohol-related cues in young binge drinkers: an event-related potentials study. Clin Neurophysiol 123: 925-936. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.10.042
    [41] Carretié L, Hinojosa JA, Martín-Loeches M, et al. (2004) Automatic attention to emotional stimuli: Neural correlates. Hum Brain Mapp 22: 290-299. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20037
    [42] Compton RJ (2003) The interface between emotion and attention: A review of evidence from psychology and neuroscience. Behav Cogn Neuroscience Rev 2: 115-129. doi: 10.1177/1534582303002002003
    [43] Cahalan D, Cisin IH, Crossley HM (1969) American drinking practices: a national study of drinking behavior and attitudes, Monograph 6. New Jersey: Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies.
    [44] Mann RE, Sobell LC, Sobell MB, et al. (1985) Reliability of a family tree questionnaire for assessing family history of alcohol problems. Drug Alcohol Depend 15:61-67.
    [45] Cranford JA, McCabe SE, Boyd CJ (2006) A new measure of binge drinking: prevalence and correlates in a probability sample of undergraduates. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 30: 1896-1905.
    [46] Fromme K, Stroot EA, Kaplan D (1993) Comprehensive effects of alcohol: development and psychometric assessment of a new expectancy questionnaire. Psychol Assess 5: 19-26.
    [47] Cuellar I, Arnold B, Gonzalez G (1995) Cognitive referents of acculturation: assessment of cultural constructs in Mexican Americans. J Community Psychol 23: 339-356.
    [48] Triandis HC, Marin G, Betancourt H, et al. (1982) Dimensions of familism among Hispanic and mainstream Navy recruits. Chicago: Department of Psychology, University of Illinois.
    [49] Paniagua FA (1994) Assessing and treating culturally diverse clients: a practical guide. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
    [50] Grebler L, Moore JW, Guzman RC (1970) The Mexican American people. New York: The Free Press.
    [51] Rodriguez Holguin S, Porjesz B, Chorlian DB, et al. (1999) Visual P3a in male subjects at high risk for alcoholism. Biol Psychiatry 46: 281-291.
    [52] DeSchepper B, Treisman A (1996) Visual memory for novel shapes: implicit coding without attention. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 22: 27-47.
    [53] Mogg K, Bradley BP, Miles F, et al. (2004) Time course of attentional bias for threat scenes: testing the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis. Cogn Emot 18: 689-700.
    [54] Semlitsch HV, Anderer P, Schuster P, et al. (1986) A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology 23: 695-703. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00696.x
    [55] Compumedics Neuroscan (2003) Scan 4.3, Volume II. El Paso, TX: Compumedics Neuroscan.
    [56] Tym C, McMillion R, Barone S, et al. (2005) First-generation college students: a literature review. Round Rock, Texas: Texas Guaranteed Student Loan Corporation.
    [57] Caetano R, Ramisetty-Mikler S, Wallisch LS, et al. (2008) Acculturation, drinking and alcohol abuse and dependence among Hispanics in the Texas-Mexico border. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res 32: 314-321. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00576.x
    [58] Fromme K, Corbin WR, Kruse MI (2008) Behavioral risks during the transition from high school to college. Dev Psychol 44: 1497-1504.
    [59] Ramos-Sanchez L, Nichols L (2007) Self-efficacy of first-generation and non-first-generation college students: the relationship with academic performance and college adjustment. J Coll Counsel 10: 6-18.
    [60] Tym C, McMillion R, Barone S, et al. (2005) First-generation college students: a literature review. Round Rock, Texas: Texas Guaranteed Student Loan Corporation.
    [61] Thayer PB (2000) Retaining first-generation and low income students. Opp Outlook 2-8.
    [62] Park CL, Armeli S, Tennen H (2004) The daily stress and coping process and alcohol use among college students. J Stud Alcohol 65: 126-135.
    [63] Finlay AK, Ram N, Maggs JL, et al. (2012) Leisure activities, the social weekend and alcohol use: evidence from a daily study of first-year college students. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 73: 250-259.
    [64] Wechsler H, Kuo M, Lee H, et al. (2000) Environmental correlates of underage alcohol use and related problems of college students. Am J Prev Med 19:24-29.
    [65] Terenzini P, Springer L, Yaeger P, et al. (1996) First-generation college students: characteristics, experiences and cognitive development. Res Higher Ed 30: 301-315.
    [66] Laude JR, Fillmore MT (2015) Alcohol cues impair learning inhibitory signals in beer drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 39: 880-886. doi: 10.1111/acer.12690
    [67] Fischer S, Smith GT, Anderson KG, et al. (2003) Expectancy influences the operation of personality on behavior. Psychol Addict Behav 17: 108-114. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.17.2.108
    [68] Brown SA, Goldman MS, Inn AM, et al. (1980) Expectancies of reinforcement from alcohol: their domain and relation to drinking patterns. J Consult Clin Psychol 48: 419-426. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.48.4.419
    [69] Sher KJ, Trull TJ (1994) Personality and disinhibitory psychopathology: alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder. Abnorm Psychol 103: 92-102. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.103.1.92
    [70] Dager A, Stevens P, Jiantonio-Kelly R, et al. (2013) Influence of alcohol use and family history of alcoholism on neural response to alcohol cues in college drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 37 Suppl 1: E161-171.
    [71] Sher KJ, Bartholow BD, Nanda S (2001) Short- and long-term effects of fraternity and sorority membership on heavy drinkning: a social norms perspective. Psychol Addict Behav 15:42-51.
    [72] Turrisi R, Mastroleo NR, Mallett KA, et al. (2007) Examination of the mediational influences of peer norms, environmental influences and parent communications on heavy drinking in athletes and nonathletes. Psychol Addict Behav 21:453-461.
    [73] Schoenmakers TM, de Bruin M, Lux IF, et al. (2010) Clinical effectiveness of attentional bias modification training in abstinent alcoholic patients. Drug Alcohol Depend 109: 30-36.


  • Reader Comments
  • © 2016 the Author(s), licensee AIMS Press. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0)
通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
  • 1. 

    沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

  1. 本站搜索
  2. 百度学术搜索
  3. 万方数据库搜索
  4. CNKI搜索

Metrics

Article views(5014) PDF downloads(1273) Cited by(1)

Article outline

Figures and Tables

Figures(4)  /  Tables(3)

/

DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
Return
Return

Catalog